The impact of different styles of traffic safety advertisement on young drivers' explicit and implicit self-enhancement biases

This study examined the immediate psychological impact of positive and negatively framed driving advertisements on (a) deliberative and consciously reported (explicit) and (b) automatic non-conscious (implicit) self-enhancement biases in driving ability and caution (N = 150). Positively framed driving advertisements (those that showed actors modeling safe alternatives to dangerous driving) were most effective at reducing self-enhancement biases in driving ability. This effect was, however, limited to consciously accessible deliberative self-enhancement biases. Exposure to driving advertisements (either positively or negatively framed) did not significantly alter implicit, automatic self-enhancement biases (measured using a computerized reaction-time task). These findings emphasize that positively framed messages are more effective than negatively framed messages at influencing important psychological processes underlying driving behaviour, although such effects are limited, at least in their immediacy, to deliberative fast-learning (or propositional) processes. The implications of these findings for understanding the effects of driving advertisements on the different slow-learning and fast-learning social-cognitive processes underlying self-enhancement biases in young drivers' self-perceptions, and hence driver behaviour, are discussed.

[1]  Erica Weintraub Austin,et al.  Questioning the value of realism: young adults' processing of messages in alcohol‐related public service announcements and advertising , 2001 .

[2]  D M DeJoy,et al.  The optimism bias and traffic accident risk perception. , 1989, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[3]  Ray G. Taylor,et al.  Age and gender differences in perceived accident likelihood and driver competences. , 1996, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[4]  K. Witte Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model , 1992 .

[5]  R. Tay,et al.  Changing Drivers' Intentions and Behaviours Using Fear-Based Driver Fatigue Advertisements , 2002, Health marketing quarterly.

[6]  M L Matthews,et al.  Age differences in male drivers' perception of accident risk: the role of perceived driving ability. , 1986, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[7]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[8]  D Walton,et al.  Drivers' biased perceptions of speed and safety campaign messages. , 2001, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[9]  R J Donovan,et al.  Executing effective road safety advertising: are big production budgets necessary? , 1999, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[10]  Julie Hatfield,et al.  A challenge to the assumed generalizability of prediction and countermeasure for risky driving: different factors predict different risky driving behaviors. , 2007, Journal of safety research.

[11]  Timo Lajunen,et al.  Asymmetric relationship between driving and safety skills. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[12]  Ioni M. Lewis,et al.  The role of fear appeals in improving driver safety: A review of the effectiveness of fear-arousing (threat) appeals in road safety advertising. , 2007 .

[13]  Richard Tay,et al.  Examining the effectiveness of physical threats in road safety advertising: The role of the third-person effect, gender, and age , 2007 .

[14]  Hamish A Deery,et al.  Hazard and Risk Perception among Young Novice Drivers , 1999 .

[15]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method Variables and Construct Validity , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[16]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  N. Harré Risk Evaluation, Driving, and Adolescents: A Typology , 2000 .

[19]  G. Bodenhausen,et al.  Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  N. Weinstein The precaution adoption process. , 1988, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[21]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Using the Implicit Association Test to Measure Self-Esteem and Self-Concept , 2000 .

[22]  Chris G Sibley,et al.  Explicit and implicit self-enhancement biases in drivers and their relationship to driving violations and crash-risk optimism. , 2007, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[23]  M. Horswill,et al.  DRIVERS' RATINGS OF DIFFERENT COMPONENTS OF THEIR OWN DRIVING SKILL: A GREATER ILLUSION OF SUPERIORITY FOR SKILLS THAT RELATE TO ACCIDENT INVOLVEMENT , 2004 .

[24]  Robert J. Rydell,et al.  Understanding implicit and explicit attitude change: a systems of reasoning analysis. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Richard Tay,et al.  The effectiveness of enforcement and publicity campaigns on serious crashes involving young male drivers: Are drink driving and speeding similar? , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[26]  Ray Fuller,et al.  The modification of individual road user behaviour , 1991 .

[27]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[28]  F. McKenna,et al.  Factors underlying illusory self-assessment of driving skill in males and females. , 1991, Accident Analysis and Prevention.

[29]  P Delhomme,et al.  Comparing one's driving with others': assessment of abilities and frequency of offences. Evidence for a superior conformity of self-bias? , 1991, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[30]  L Quine,et al.  Perceptions of risk in motorcyclists: unrealistic optimism, relative realism and predictions of behaviour. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[31]  David D Clarke,et al.  Voluntary risk taking and skill deficits in young driver accidents in the UK. , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[32]  N. Harré,et al.  Self-enhancement, crash-risk optimism and the impact of safety advertisements on young drivers. , 2005, British journal of psychology.

[33]  K. White,et al.  Promoting Public Health Messages: Should We Move Beyond Fear-Evoking Appeals in Road Safety? , 2007, Qualitative health research.

[34]  B Fischhoff,et al.  Perceived driving safety and seatbelt usage. , 1985, Accident; analysis and prevention.