Disorientation inhibits landmark use in 12-18-month-old infants.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[2] Lynn Nadel,et al. Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.
[3] J. Gavin Bremner,et al. Use of cue configuration geometry for spatial orientation in human infants (Homo sapiens). , 2001 .
[4] L. Acredolo,et al. Developmental changes in the effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior. , 1980 .
[5] R. Muller,et al. Failure of Centrally Placed Objects to Control the Firing Fields of Hippocampal Place Cells , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[6] Simon Benhamou,et al. LANDMARK USE BY NAVIGATING RATS (RATTUS NORVEGICUS) : CONTRASTING GEOMETRIC AND FEATURAL INFORMATION , 1998 .
[7] R. Muller,et al. Further study of the control of place cell firing by intra‐apparatus objects , 1999, Hippocampus.
[8] E. Spelke,et al. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.
[9] J. Taube,et al. Effects of repeated disorientation on the acquisition of spatial tasks in rats: dissociation between the appetitive radial arm maze and aversive water maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[10] Kathryn J. Jeffery,et al. The neurobiology of spatial behaviour , 2003 .
[11] L. Acredolo. Development of Spatial Orientation in Infancy. , 1978 .
[12] J. O’Keefe,et al. Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: Why they fire where they fire , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] E. Spelke,et al. Children's use of geometry and landmarks to reorient in an open space , 2001, Cognition.
[14] E. Save,et al. Drawing parallels between the behavioural and neural properties of navigation , 2003 .
[15] N. Newcombe,et al. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[16] J. Rieser. Spatial orientation of six-month-old infants. , 1979, Child development.
[17] The development of beacon use for spatial orientation in 6–8.5-month-old infants , 2000 .
[18] G. M. Martin,et al. Spatial disorientation blocks reliable goal location on a plus maze but does not prevent goal location in the Morris maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[19] K. Cheng. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.
[20] Adina R. Lew,et al. Indirect landmark use at 6 months of age in a spatial orientation task. , 2004 .
[21] R. F. Wang,et al. Comparative approaches to human navigation , 2003 .
[22] E. Spelke,et al. Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .
[23] R H Day,et al. Spatial localization in infancy: position constancy in a square and circular room with and without a landmark. , 1986, Child development.
[24] J. G. Bremner,et al. The development of relational landmark use in six- to twelve-month-old infants in a spatial orientation task. , 2000, Child development.
[25] L. Nadel,et al. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .
[26] E. Bushnell,et al. The spatial coding strategies of one-year-old infants in a locomotor search task. , 1995, Child development.