Numerical and nonnumerical estimation in children with and without mathematical learning disabilities

There are currently multiple explanations for mathematical learning disabilities (MLD). The present study focused on those assuming that MLD are due to a basic numerical deficit affecting the ability to represent and to manipulate number magnitude (Butterworth, 1999, 2005; A. J. Wilson & Dehaene, 2007) and/or to access that number magnitude representation from numerical symbols (Rousselle & Noël, 2007). The present study provides an original contribution to this issue by testing MLD children (carefully selected on the basis of preserved abilities in other domains) on numerical estimation tasks with contrasting symbolic (Arabic numerals) and nonsymbolic (collection of dots) numbers used as input or output. MLD children performed consistently less accurately than control children on all the estimation tasks. However, MLD children were even weaker when the task involved the mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers than when the task required a mapping between two nonsymbolic numerical formats. Moreover, in the estimation of nonsymbolic numerosities, MLD children relied more than control children on perceptual cues such as the cumulative area of the dots. Finally, the task requiring a mapping from a nonsymbolic format to a symbolic format was the best predictor of MLD. In order to explain these present results, as well as those reported in the literature, we propose that the impoverished number magnitude representation of MLD children may arise from an initial mapping deficit between number symbols and that magnitude representation.

[1]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  A longitudinal study of mathematical competencies in children with specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid mathematics and reading difficulties. , 2003, Child development.

[2]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Core information processing deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. , 2008, Developmental science.

[4]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students , 2004, Cognition.

[5]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Numerical estimation in preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[6]  R. Shalev Prevalence of developmental dyscalculia. , 2007 .

[7]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[8]  J. Sattler,et al.  Assessment of Children's Intelligence and Special Abilities , 1981 .

[9]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[10]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[12]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  Carole S. Robinson,et al.  Toward a Two–Factor Theory of One Type of Mathematics Disabilities , 2002 .

[14]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  How understanding mathematical learning disability may guide mathematics teaching , 2012 .

[15]  David C. Geary,et al.  Development of Number Line Representations in Children With Mathematical Learning Disability , 2008, Developmental neuropsychology.

[16]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .

[17]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Calibrating the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.

[18]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[20]  Orly Manor,et al.  Developmental dyscalculia: a prospective six‐year follow‐up , 2005, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[21]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .

[22]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Under- and over-estimation: a bi-directional mapping process between symbolic and non-symbolic representations of number? , 2011, Experimental psychology.

[23]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[24]  C. Gilmore,et al.  Children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities , 2007 .

[26]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[28]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[29]  R. Shalev,et al.  Developmental Dyscalculia , 2004, Journal of child neurology.

[30]  Laurence Rousselle,et al.  Mental Arithmetic in Children With Mathematics Learning Disabilities , 2008, Journal of learning disabilities.

[31]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  The development of automatic numerosity processing in preschoolers: evidence for numerosity-perceptual interference. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[32]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Numerical estimation in blind subjects: evidence of the impact of blindness and its following experience. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  Justin N Wood,et al.  Infants' enumeration of actions: numerical discrimination and its signature limits. , 2005, Developmental science.

[34]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Discrimination of Large and Small Numerosities by Human Infants , 2004 .

[35]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[36]  Kristine L Koontz,et al.  Identifying Simple Numerical Stimuli: Processing Inefficiencies Exhibited by Arithmetic Learning Disabled Children , 1996 .

[37]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. , 2007, Child development.

[38]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[39]  Herbert P. Ginsburg,et al.  Cognitive Analysis of Children's Mathematics Difficulties , 1984 .

[40]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[41]  B. Rourke,et al.  Disabilities of Arithmetic and Mathematical Reasoning , 1997, Journal of learning disabilities.

[42]  D. Geary Mathematics and Learning Disabilities , 2004, Journal of learning disabilities.

[43]  M. von Aster,et al.  Number development and developmental dyscalculia , 2007, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[44]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Numerical and arithmetical cognition: patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[45]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Magnitude comparison in preschoolers: what counts? Influence of perceptual variables. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[46]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: is number one of them? , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[47]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' Discrimination of Number vs. Continuous Extent , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[49]  John R. Platt,et al.  Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: Effects of error contingencies , 1971 .

[50]  Gavin R. Price,et al.  Impaired parietal magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia , 2007, Current Biology.

[51]  J. Wilson,et al.  Developmental differences in the span of visual memory for pattern , 1987 .

[52]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[53]  D. Cuneo,et al.  Children's judgments of numerical quantity: A new view of early quantification , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[54]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Developmental Dyscalculia: heterogeneity might not mean different mechanisms , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Performance across different areas of mathematical cognition in children with learning difficulties. , 2001 .

[56]  D. Geary Mathematical disabilities: cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[58]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[59]  Gavin Huntley-Fenner,et al.  Children's understanding of number is similar to adults' and rats': numerical estimation by 5–7-year-olds , 2001, Cognition.