Imperfect optics may be the eye's defence against chromatic blur

The optics of the eye cause different wavelengths of light to be differentially focused at the retina. This phenomenon is due to longitudinal chromatic aberration, a wavelength-dependent change in refractive power. Retinal image quality may consequently vary for the different classes of cone photoreceptors, cells tuned to absorb bands of different wavelengths. For instance, it has been assumed that when the eye is focused for mid-spectral wavelengths near the peak sensitivities of long- (L) and middle- (M) wavelength-sensitive cones, short-wavelength (bluish) light is so blurred that it cannot contribute to and may even impair spatial vision. These optical effects have been proposed to explain the function of the macular pigment, which selectively absorbs short-wavelength light, and the sparsity of short-wavelength-sensitive (S) cones. However, such explanations have ignored the effect of monochromatic wave aberrations present in real eyes. Here we show that, when these effects are taken into account, short wavelengths are not as blurred as previously thought, that the potential image quality for S cones is comparable to that for L and M cones, and that macular pigment has no significant function in improving the retinal image.

[1]  D. Williams,et al.  Monochromatic aberrations of the human eye in a large population. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[2]  D. Macleod,et al.  Spectral sensitivities of the human cones. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[3]  Stephen A. Burns,et al.  A new approach to the study of ocular chromatic aberrations , 1999, Vision Research.

[4]  A. Bradley,et al.  Spherical Aberration of the Reduced Schematic Eye with Elliptical Refracting Surface , 1997, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[5]  S A Burns,et al.  Age-related changes in monochromatic wave aberrations of the human eye. , 2001, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[6]  R. Bone,et al.  Lutein, zeaxanthin, and the macular pigment. , 2001, Archives of biochemistry and biophysics.

[7]  D. Whitaker,et al.  Factors affecting light-adapted pupil size in normal human subjects. , 1994, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[8]  J. Mollon "Tho' she kneel'd in that place where they grew..." The uses and origins of primate colour vision. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[9]  John D. Mollon,et al.  A TAXONOMY OF TRITANOPIAS , 1982 .

[10]  Leo Maurice Hurvich,et al.  Color vision , 1981 .

[11]  G. Wyszecki,et al.  Axial chromatic aberration of the human eye. , 1957, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[12]  R. M. Boynton Human color vision , 1979 .

[13]  G. Haegerstrom‐Portnoy,et al.  Short-wavelength-sensitive-cone sensitivity loss with aging: a protective role for macular pigment? , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[14]  D M Snodderly,et al.  Evidence for protection against age-related macular degeneration by carotenoids and antioxidant vitamins. , 1995, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[15]  Susana Marcos,et al.  Investigating sources of variability of monochromatic and transverse chromatic aberrations across eyes , 2001, Vision Research.

[16]  Gunther Wyszecki,et al.  Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition , 2000 .

[17]  L N Thibos,et al.  Statistical distribution of foveal transverse chromatic aberration, pupil centration, and angle psi in a population of young adult eyes. , 1995, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[18]  R A Weale,et al.  Macular pigment and chromatic aberration. , 1974, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[19]  J. Cohen,et al.  Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulas , 1968 .