Motor learning without doing: trial-by-trial improvement in motor performance during mental training.

Although there is converging experimental and clinical evidences suggesting that mental training with motor imagery can improve motor performance, it is unclear how humans can learn movements through mental training despite the lack of sensory feedback from the body and the environment. In a first experiment, we measured the trial-by-trial decrease in durations of executed movements (physical training group) and mentally simulated movements (motor-imagery training group), by means of training on a multiple-target arm-pointing task requiring high accuracy and speed. Movement durations were significantly lower in posttest compared with pretest after both physical and motor-imagery training. Although both the posttraining performance and the rate of learning were smaller in motor-imagery training group than in physical training group, the change in movement duration and the asymptotic movement duration after a hypothetical large number of trials were identical. The two control groups (eye-movement training and rest groups) did not show change in movement duration. In the second experiment, additional kinematic analyses revealed that arm movements were straighter and faster both immediately and 24 h after physical and motor-imagery training. No such improvements were observed in the eye-movement training group. Our results suggest that the brain uses state estimation, provided by internal forward model predictions, to improve motor performance during mental training. Furthermore, our results suggest that mental practice can, at least in young healthy subjects and if given after a short bout of physical practice, be successfully substituted to physical practice to improve motor performance.

[1]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Comparison of actual and imagined execution of whole-body movements after a long exposure to microgravity , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[3]  Michael F. Land,et al.  From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[5]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation , 2003, Nature.

[6]  P. Maruff,et al.  Asymmetries between dominant and non-dominanthands in real and imagined motor task performance , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  D. Willingham,et al.  Patterns of interference in sequence learning and prism adaptation inconsistent with the consolidation hypothesis. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[8]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Andrea Brovelli,et al.  Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  E. G. Hall,et al.  The Effect of Visuo-Motor Behavior Rehearsal with Videotaped Modeling on Free Throw Accuracy of Intercollegiate Female Basketball Players , 1983 .

[11]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Consolidation of motor memory , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Practice with Sleep Makes Perfect Sleep-Dependent Motor Skill Learning , 2002, Neuron.

[13]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Motor prediction , 2001, Current Biology.

[14]  G. Courtine,et al.  Gait-dependent motor memory facilitation in covert movement execution. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  M. Desmurget,et al.  Movement Intention After Parietal Cortex Stimulation in Humans , 2009, Science.

[16]  Steven C Cramer,et al.  Cortical Plasticity Following Motor Skill Learning During Mental Practice in Stroke , 2009, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[17]  M. Ghilardi,et al.  Implicit and explicit aspects of sequence learning in pre-symptomatic Huntington's disease. , 2008, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[18]  Paul Maruff,et al.  The effect of an external load on the force and timing components of mentally represented actions , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[20]  Christopher M. Bishop,et al.  Neural networks for pattern recognition , 1995 .

[21]  E. Wist,et al.  A mental route to motor learning: Improving trajectorial kinematics through imagery training , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[22]  K. Zentgraf,et al.  Cognitive motor processes: The role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations , 2009, Brain Research Reviews.

[23]  C. Richards,et al.  Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. , 2001, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[24]  Stefan Geyer,et al.  Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Bradley P. Carlin,et al.  BAYES AND EMPIRICAL BAYES METHODS FOR DATA ANALYSIS , 1996, Stat. Comput..

[26]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  "Pre-symptomatic" Huntington's disease. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[27]  S. Scott,et al.  A motor learning strategy reflects neural circuitry for limb control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[29]  R. Johansson,et al.  Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.

[30]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[32]  Andrea Tacchino,et al.  Spontaneous movement tempo is influenced by observation of rhythmical actions , 2009, Brain Research Bulletin.

[33]  Julien Doyon,et al.  Functional cerebral reorganization following motor sequence learning through mental practice with motor imagery , 2003, NeuroImage.

[34]  R. Johansson,et al.  Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.

[35]  C. Hall,et al.  Measuring movement imagery abilities: A revision of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire. , 1997 .

[36]  L. Christensen,et al.  University of Birmingham Disruption of state estimation in the human lateral cerebellum , 2007 .

[37]  D. Rainey,et al.  Mental Imagery, Relaxation, and Accuracy of Basketball Foul Shooting , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[38]  T. Pozzo,et al.  Improvement and generalization of arm motor performance through motor imagery practice , 2006, Neuroscience.

[39]  R. Gentili,et al.  Inertial properties of the arm are accurately predicted during motor imagery , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[40]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[41]  Jing Z. Liu,et al.  From mental power to muscle power—gaining strength by using the mind , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Motor Learning Produces Parallel Dynamic Functional Changes during the Execution and Imagination of Sequential Foot Movements , 2002, NeuroImage.

[43]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[46]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[47]  Elizabeth L Shoenfelt,et al.  Evaluation of a Mental Skills Program for Serving for an Intercollegiate Volleyball Team , 2008, Perceptual and motor skills.

[48]  John W Krakauer,et al.  Learning of a sequential motor skill comprises explicit and implicit components that consolidate differently. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  M. Arbib,et al.  Role of the cerebellum in reaching movements in humans. I. Distributed inverse dynamics control , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[50]  G. Tononi,et al.  The serial reaction time task revisited: a study on motor sequence learning with an arm-reaching task , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  S. Schaal,et al.  Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  Kenji Doya,et al.  Humans Can Adopt Optimal Discounting Strategy under Real-Time Constraints , 2006, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[53]  F. P. de Lange,et al.  Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  J. Doyon,et al.  Sleep-related improvements in motor learning following mental practice , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[55]  Marco Schieppati,et al.  Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under different conditions of direction and mass , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.