Automatic imitation of multiple agents: A computational model
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Brass,et al. Automatic Imitation of Multiple Agents: Simultaneous or Random Representation? , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] Xiucheng Fan,et al. Group Emotional Contagion and Complaint Intentions in Group Service Failure , 2014 .
[3] A. Bayliss,et al. Gaze following in multiagent contexts: Evidence for a quorum-like principle , 2018, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] G. Bird,et al. Sensorimotor training alters action understanding , 2018, Cognition.
[6] R. Bogacz,et al. The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.
[7] T. Chartrand,et al. The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.
[8] S. Tipper,et al. Attention modulates motor system activation during action observation: evidence for inhibitory rebound , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] Emily E. Butler,et al. Investigating the Relationship between Stable Personality Characteristics and Automatic Imitation , 2015, PloS one.
[10] M. Brass,et al. Automatic imitation of pro- and antisocial gestures: Is implicit social behavior censored? , 2018, Cognition.
[11] M. Heene,et al. The bystander-effect: a meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.
[12] Matthew J. C. Crump,et al. In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[13] M. Brass,et al. Mirroring multiple agents: motor resonance during action observation is modulated by the number of agents. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[14] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.
[15] Marcel Brass,et al. Experience-based priming of body parts: A study of action imitation , 2008, Brain Research.
[16] T. Chartrand,et al. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[17] W. Prinz,et al. Compatibility between Observed and Executed Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and Imitative Cues , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[18] Daniel Yoshor,et al. Receptive fields in human visual cortex mapped with surface electrodes. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[19] M. Brass,et al. Mimicry and automatic imitation are not correlated , 2017, PloS one.
[20] Lincoln J. Colling,et al. How does “mirroring” support joint action? , 2013, Cortex.
[21] Matthias Scheutz,et al. In Praise of a Model but Not Its Conclusions: Commentary on Cooper, Catmur, and Heyes (2012) , 2013, Cogn. Sci..
[22] Nick Chater,et al. Herding in humans , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] Caroline Catmur,et al. Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] E. S. Knowles,et al. Groups and crowds as social entities: Effects of activity, size, and member similarity on nonmembers. , 1976 .
[25] M. Brass,et al. Motor simulation beyond the dyad: Automatic imitation of multiple actors. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] W. Notebaert,et al. Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. , 2016, Psychological bulletin.
[27] S. Kornblum,et al. A Parallel Distributed Processing Model of Stimulus–Stimulus and Stimulus–Response Compatibility , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.
[28] Paul E. Downing,et al. Facilitation and interference in spatial and body reference frames , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[29] Béatrice de Gelder,et al. The neural basis of the bystander effect — The influence of group size on neural activity when witnessing an emergency , 2014, NeuroImage.
[30] Simon Garnier,et al. Visual attention and the acquisition of information in human crowds , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[31] Daniel C. Richardson,et al. Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in autism spectrum conditions , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[32] M. Brass,et al. Automatic Imitation: A Meta-Analysis , 2018, Psychological bulletin.
[33] B. Latané. The psychology of social impact. , 1981 .
[34] B. Pillon,et al. Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients , 1986, Annals of neurology.
[35] Rainer Goebel,et al. The brain's intention to imitate: The neurobiology of intentional versus automatic imitation , 2008, Brain Stimulation.
[36] Jan Derrfuss,et al. Imitative response tendencies in patients with frontal brain lesions. , 2003, Neuropsychology.
[37] M. Zorzi,et al. A computational model of the Simon effect , 1995, Psychological research.
[38] E. Renzi,et al. Imitation and utilisation behaviour. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[39] N. Sebanz,et al. The GROOP effect: Groups mimic group actions , 2011, Cognition.
[40] G. Alvarez. Representing multiple objects as an ensemble enhances visual cognition , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] A. Greenwald,et al. Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. , 1970, Psychological review.
[42] C. Heyes,et al. MIRROR NEURONS: TESTS AND TESTABILITY , 2014 .
[43] Y. Paulignan,et al. An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.
[44] Matthias Scheutz,et al. A Computational PDP Model for Explaining Automatic Imitation , 2012, CogSci.
[45] Mowei Shen,et al. Perceiving crowd attention: Gaze following in human crowds with conflicting cues , 2017, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[46] James Stanley,et al. Effects of Agency on Movement Interference During Observation of a Moving Dot Stimulus , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[47] Robert J MacCoun,et al. The Burden of Social Proof: Shared Thresholds and Social Influence , 2011, Psychological review.
[48] Aude Billard,et al. Parallel and distributed neural models of the ideomotor principle: An investigation of imitative cortical pathways , 2006, Neural Networks.
[49] C. Heyes,et al. Hand to mouth: automatic imitation across effector systems. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[50] G. Knoblich,et al. The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.
[51] Caroline Catmur,et al. Are Automatic Imitation and Spatial Compatibility Mediated by Different Processes? , 2013, Cogn. Sci..
[52] C. Heyes. Where do mirror neurons come from? , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[53] R. Bond,et al. Group Size and Conformity , 2005 .
[54] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[55] G Aschersleben,et al. Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] B. Hommel,et al. HiTEC: a connectionist model of the interaction between perception and action planning , 2016, Psychological research.
[57] M. Brass,et al. Motor simulation of multiple observed actions , 2018, Cognition.
[58] G. Pourtois,et al. The time course of cognitive control implementation , 2016, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[59] Seth A. Herd,et al. A Unified Framework for Inhibitory Control Opinion , 2022 .
[60] B. Latané,et al. Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[61] M. Brass,et al. Representing Multiple Observed Actions in the Motor System , 2018, bioRxiv.
[62] S. Milgram,et al. Note on the drawing power of crowds of different size. , 1969 .
[63] Verónica C. Ramenzoni,et al. Scaling-up Perception-Action Links: Evidence from Synchronization with Individual and Joint Action , 2013, CogSci.
[64] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[65] K. Grill-Spector,et al. Relating retinotopic and object-selective responses in human lateral occipital cortex. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[66] J. Maunsell,et al. Anterior inferotemporal neurons of monkeys engaged in object recognition can be highly sensitive to object retinal position. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[67] M. Brass,et al. Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[68] M. Brass,et al. The role of sensorimotor processes in social group contagion , 2018, Cognitive Psychology.
[69] W. Prinz. Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .
[70] Angela R. Laird,et al. ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain , 2010, NeuroImage.
[71] A. Dickinson,et al. Associative (not Hebbian) learning and the mirror neuron system , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.