Event-related potentials elicited by errors during the stop-signal task. II: human effector-specific error responses.

Although previous research with human and nonhuman primates has examined the neural correlates of performance monitoring, discrepancies in methodology have limited our ability to make cross-species generalizations. One major obstacle arises from the use of different behavioral responses and tasks across different primate species. Specifically, it is unknown whether performance-monitoring mechanisms rely on different neural circuitry in tasks requiring oculomotor vs. skeletomotor responses. Here, we show that the human error-related negativity (ERN) elicited by a saccadic eye-movement response relative to a manual response differs in several critical ways. The human saccadic ERN exhibits a prolonged duration, a broader frontomedial voltage distribution, and different neural source estimates than the manual ERN in exactly the same stop-signal task. The human saccadic error positivity (Pe) exhibited a frontomedial voltage distribution with estimated electrical sources in supplementary motor area and rostral anterior cingulate cortex for saccadic responses, whereas the manual Pe showed a posterior scalp distribution and potential origins in the superior parietal lobule. These findings constrain models of the cognitive mechanisms indexed by the ERN/Pe complex. Moreover, by paralleling work with nonhuman primates performing the same saccadic stop-signal task (Godlove et al. 2011), we demonstrate a cross-species homology of error event-related potentials (ERPs) and lay the groundwork for definitively localizing the neural sources of performance-monitoring ERPs.

[1]  B. Kopp,et al.  N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[2]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Error-related scalp potentials elicited by hand and foot movements: evidence for an output-independent error-processing system in humans , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[3]  Robert Roman,et al.  Intracerebral Error-Related Negativity in a Simple Go/NoGo Task , 2005 .

[4]  M. Herrmann,et al.  Source localization (LORETA) of the error-related-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe). , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys , 2007, Vision Research.

[6]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[7]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Theodore H. Poister,et al.  Performance monitoring , 1996 .

[9]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  J. Schall,et al.  Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye ® eld , 2000 .

[12]  S. Luck An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique , 2005 .

[13]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  E. Donchin,et al.  Event-related brain potentials and error-related processing: an analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[15]  S. Baldwin,et al.  Empathy and error processing. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[16]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  A Model of How the Basal Ganglia Generate and Use Neural Signals That Predict Reinforcement , 1994 .

[17]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  M. Fuchs,et al.  A standardized boundary element method volume conductor model , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[19]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Conflict in Cingulate Cortex Function between Humans and Macaque Monkeys: More Apparent than Real , 2010, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[20]  Scott A Baldwin,et al.  Temporal stability of the error-related negativity (ERN) and post-error positivity (Pe): the role of number of trials. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[21]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Role of the human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor, manual, and speech responses: a positron emission tomography study. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  D. Tucker,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex in Action Monitoring , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  G. Logan,et al.  Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control , 1997 .

[24]  N. Yeung,et al.  On the ERN and the significance of errors. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[25]  M. Roesch,et al.  Neuronal activity in macaque SEF and ACC during performance of tasks involving conflict. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  B. Kopp,et al.  An event-related brain potential substrate of disturbed response monitoring in paranoid schizophrenic patients. , 1999, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[27]  P. Apkarian,et al.  Motoric response inhibition in finger movement and saccadic eye movement: a comparative study , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Countermanding saccades in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[29]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Cingulate input to the primary and supplementary motor cortices in the rhesus monkey: Evidence for somatotopy in areas 24c and 23c , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  M. Coles,et al.  "Where did I go wrong?" A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[32]  A. Starr,et al.  Task-relevant late positive component of the auditory event-related potential in monkeys resembles P300 in humans. , 1984, Science.

[33]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-signal Paradigm Stop-signal Paradigm Post-stop-signal Slowing , 2011 .

[35]  J. Kaas The Evolution of Visual Cortex in Primates , 2006 .

[36]  Do patients with damage to the anterior cingulate and adjacent regions produce an error-related negativity (ERN)? , 2000 .

[37]  Tanaka The role of , 2000, Journal of insect physiology.

[38]  William H. Alexander,et al.  Computational Models of Performance Monitoring and Cognitive Control , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[39]  W. Modell On the significance of significant , 1981, The New England journal of medicine.

[40]  W. Schultz Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[41]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[42]  G. Logan,et al.  Development of inhibitory control across the life span. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[43]  P. Jurák,et al.  Error processing – evidence from intracerebral ERP recordings , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  William J. Gehring,et al.  The Error-Related Negativity (ERN/Ne) , 2011 .

[45]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[46]  Marta Kutas,et al.  The intractability of scaling scalp distributions to infer neuroelectric sources. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[47]  D. Pandya,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[48]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Conflict over Cingulate Cortex: Between-Species Differences in Cingulate May Support Enhanced Cognitive Flexibility in Humans , 2010, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[49]  A Berthoz,et al.  A positron emission tomography study of oculomotor imagery. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[50]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[52]  V. Stuphorn,et al.  Supplementary Motor Area Exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[54]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades with auditory stop signals: testing the race model , 2001, Vision Research.

[55]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[56]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  T. Paus,et al.  Regional differences in the effects of task difficulty and motor output on blood flow response in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a review of 107 PET activation studies , 1998, Neuroreport.

[58]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[59]  E. Harth,et al.  Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of Eeg , 2005 .

[60]  A. Barto,et al.  Adaptive Critics and the Basal Ganglia , 1994 .

[61]  T. Endrass,et al.  Error Awareness in a Saccade Countermanding Task , 2005 .

[62]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Performance monitoring local field potentials in the medial frontal cortex of primates: supplementary eye field. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Nonindependent and nonstationary response times in stopping and stepping saccade tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[64]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[65]  Jutta Stahl,et al.  Dynamics of response-conflict monitoring and individual differences in response control and behavioral control: An electrophysiological investigation using a stop-signal task , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[66]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Don’t look! don’t touch! inhibitory control of eye and hand movements , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[67]  G. Logan,et al.  Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[68]  Patricia E Pailing,et al.  Error negativity and response control. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[69]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[70]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Performance Monitoring by the Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Saccade Countermanding , 2003, Science.

[71]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas in the frontal lobe of the primate , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[72]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[73]  J. Fermaglich Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEG , 1982 .

[74]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[75]  D. Tucker,et al.  Electrophysiological Responses to Errors and Feedback in the Process of Action Regulation , 2003, Psychological science.

[76]  Dirk J. Heslenfeld,et al.  Adaptive control deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The role of error processing , 2007, Psychiatry Research.

[77]  Jeffrey D Schall,et al.  Event-Related Potentials Elicited by Errors during the Stop-Signal Task. I. Macaque Monkeys , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[78]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades: Evidence against independent processing of go and stop signals , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[79]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Saccadic countermanding: a comparison of central and peripheral stop signals , 2001, Vision Research.

[80]  Wim Fias,et al.  Outcome expectancy and not accuracy determines posterror slowing: ERP support , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[81]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[82]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Differential involvement of the aterior cingulated cortex in performance monitoring during a stop-signal task , 2005 .

[83]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[84]  C. C. Wood,et al.  The ɛ-Adjustment Procedure for Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance , 1976 .

[85]  A. Turken,et al.  Response selection in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[86]  Jeffrey D Schall,et al.  Performance monitoring local field potentials in the medial frontal cortex of primates: supplementary eye field. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[87]  W. Gehring,et al.  Functions of the Medial Frontal Cortex in the Processing of Conflict and Errors , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[88]  G. Woodman A brief introduction to the use of event-related potentials in studies of perception and attention. , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[89]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Cingulate cortex: Diverging data from humans and monkeys , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[90]  V. Stuphorn,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex Motivates But Does Not Control Movement Initiation in the Countermanding Task , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[91]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.