Visual Feedback Is Not Necessary for the Learning of Novel Dynamics

Background When learning to perform a novel sensorimotor task, humans integrate multi-modal sensory feedback such as vision and proprioception in order to make the appropriate adjustments to successfully complete the task. Sensory feedback is used both during movement to control and correct the current movement, and to update the feed-forward motor command for subsequent movements. Previous work has shown that adaptation to stable dynamics is possible without visual feedback. However, it is not clear to what degree visual information during movement contributes to this learning or whether it is essential to the development of an internal model or impedance controller. Methodology/Principle Findings We examined the effects of the removal of visual feedback during movement on the learning of both stable and unstable dynamics in comparison with the case when both vision and proprioception are available. Subjects were able to learn to make smooth movements in both types of novel dynamics after learning with or without visual feedback. By examining the endpoint stiffness and force after learning it could be shown that subjects adapted to both types of dynamics in the same way whether they were provided with visual feedback of their trajectory or not. The main effects of visual feedback were to increase the success rate of movements, slightly straighten the path, and significantly reduce variability near the end of the movement. Conclusions/Significance These findings suggest that visual feedback of the hand during movement is not necessary for the adaptation to either stable or unstable novel dynamics. Instead vision appears to be used to fine-tune corrections of hand trajectory at the end of reaching movements.

[1]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  N. Hogan Adaptive control of mechanical impedance by coactivation of antagonist muscles , 1984 .

[3]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[6]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[7]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Quantitative evaluation of the electromyographic responses to multidirectional load perturbations of the human arm. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  C. Gielen,et al.  Long‐latency stretch reflexes as co‐ordinated functional responses in man. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  T. Milner,et al.  The effect of accuracy constraints on three-dimensional movement kinematics , 1990, Neuroscience.

[10]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[11]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  P Cordo,et al.  Proprioceptive coordination of movement sequences: role of velocity and position information. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  C Ghez,et al.  Proprioceptive control of interjoint coordination. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[16]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[18]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.

[20]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Human arm stiffness and equilibrium-point trajectory during multi-joint movement , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning , 1997, Nature.

[23]  Stephen H. Scott,et al.  Hand and joint paths during reaching movements with and without vision , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  H. Gomi,et al.  Task-Dependent Viscoelasticity of Human Multijoint Arm and Its Spatial Characteristics for Interaction with Environments , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Quantization of human motions and learning of accurate movements , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[26]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  M. Graziano Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Context estimation for sensorimotor control. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  J. Lackner,et al.  Congenitally blind individuals rapidly adapt to coriolis force perturbations of their reaching movements. , 2000, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[30]  G. F. Koshland,et al.  Electromyographic responses to a mechanical perturbation applied during impending arm movements in different directions: one-joint and two-joint conditions , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  E Burdet,et al.  A method for measuring endpoint stiffness during multi-joint arm movements. , 2000, Journal of biomechanics.

[32]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Impedance control and internal model formation when reaching in a randomly varying dynamical environment. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Rieko Osu,et al.  The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance , 2001, Nature.

[34]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[35]  D. Wolpert,et al.  When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.

[36]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Kinematics and Dynamics Are Not Represented Independently in Motor Working Memory: Evidence from an Interference Study , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Rieko Osu,et al.  Different mechanisms involved in adaptation to stable and unstable dynamics. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differential contributions of vision and proprioception to movement accuracy , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  J. Saunders,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast corrections of movements with a computer mouse. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[41]  M. Kawato,et al.  Adaptation to Stable and Unstable Dynamics Achieved By Combined Impedance Control and Inverse Dynamics Model , 2003 .

[42]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  M. Kawato,et al.  Functional significance of stiffness in adaptation of multijoint arm movements to stable and unstable dynamics , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  Neville Hogan,et al.  The mechanics of multi-joint posture and movement control , 1985, Biological Cybernetics.

[45]  B. Edin,et al.  Task requirements influence sensory integration during grasping in humans. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[46]  M. Kawato,et al.  Impedance control balances stability with metabolically costly muscle activation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  J. Vercher,et al.  Online control of the direction of rapid reaching movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  David C Knill,et al.  Visual Feedback Control of Hand Movements , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  R. Miall,et al.  The curvature of human arm movements in the absence of visual experience , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  P. Bédard,et al.  On-line vs. off-line utilization of peripheral visual afferent information to ensure spatial accuracy of goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  M. Kawato,et al.  Random presentation enables subjects to adapt to two opposing forces on the hand , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Failure to Consolidate the Consolidation Theory of Learning for Sensorimotor Adaptation Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Iven M. Y. Mareels,et al.  Stability and motor adaptation in human arm movements , 2005, Biological Cybernetics.

[55]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Impedance is modulated to meet accuracy demands during goal-directed arm movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[58]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Can co-activation reduce kinematic variability? A simulation study , 2005, Biological Cybernetics.

[59]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Determinants of Offline Processing of Visual Information for the Control of Reaching Movements , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[60]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Updating of an internal model without proprioception: a deafferentation study , 2006, Neuroreport.

[61]  Rieko Osu,et al.  Endpoint Stiffness of the Arm Is Directionally Tuned to Instability in the Environment , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  David J Ostry,et al.  Neural averaging in motor learning. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.