Reorganization of finger coordination patterns during adaptation to rotation and scaling of a newly learned sensorimotor transformation.

We examined how people organize redundant kinematic control variables (finger joint configurations) while learning to make goal-directed movements of a virtual object (a cursor) within a low-dimensional task space (a computer screen). Subjects participated in three experiments performed on separate days. Learning progressed rapidly on day 1, resulting in reduced target capture error and increased cursor trajectory linearity. On days 2 and 3, one group of subjects adapted to a rotation of the nominal map, imposed either stepwise or randomly over trials. Another group experienced a scaling distortion. We report two findings. First, adaptation rates and memory-dependent motor command updating depended on distortion type. Stepwise application and removal of the rotation induced a marked increase in finger motion variability but scaling did not, suggesting that the rotation initiated a more exhaustive search through the space of viable finger motions to resolve the target capture task than did scaling. Indeed, subjects formed new coordination patterns in compensating the rotation but relied on patterns established during baseline practice to compensate the scaling. These findings support the idea that the brain compensates direction and extent errors separately and in computationally distinct ways, but are inconsistent with the idea that once a task is learned, command updating is limited to those degrees of freedom contributing to performance (thereby minimizing energetic or similar costs of control). Second, we report that subjects who learned a scaling while moving to just one target generalized more narrowly across directions than those who learned a rotation. This contrasts with results from whole-arm reaching studies, where a learned scaling generalizes more broadly across direction than rotation. Based on inverse- and forward-dynamics analyses of reaching with the arm, we propose the difference in results derives from extensive exposure in reaching with familiar arm dynamics versus the novelty of the manual task.

[1]  Raul Benitez,et al.  Motor adaptation as a greedy optimization of error and effort. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Why Don't We Move Faster? Parkinson's Disease, Movement Vigor, and Implicit Motivation , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Biological constraints simplify the recognition of hand shapes , 2003, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[4]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  F. Horak,et al.  Influence of the globus pallidus on arm movements in monkeys. III. Timing of movement-related information. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Persistence of motor adaptation during constrained, multi-joint, arm movements. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  M. E. Anderson,et al.  Pallidal discharge related to the kinematics of reaching movements in two dimensions. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  K. Doya Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[9]  J. Diedrichsen Optimal Task-Dependent Changes of Bimanual Feedback Control and Adaptation , 2007, Current Biology.

[10]  K. Kurata,et al.  Premotor cortex of monkeys: set- and movement-related activity reflecting amplitude and direction of wrist movements. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  W. T. Thach A Role for the Cerebellum in Learning Movement Coordination , 1998, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[12]  Hagai Bergman,et al.  Acquisition and generalization of visuomotor transformations by nonhuman primates , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Basal ganglia network mediates the control of movement amplitude , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Postural Hand Synergies for Tool Use , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Gwilym M. Jenkins,et al.  Time series analysis, forecasting and control , 1971 .

[16]  J. Houk,et al.  Deciding when and how to correct a movement: discrete submovements as a decision making process , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Motor learning: changes in the structure of variability in a redundant task. , 2009, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[18]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[19]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Remapping hand movements in a novel geometrical environment. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Julie Messier,et al.  Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  J. Wallace Nygard,et al.  Cerebral circulation prevailing during sleep and hypnosis , 1939 .

[22]  M. Kawato,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[25]  Claude Ghez,et al.  Separate adaptive mechanisms for controlling trajectory and final position in reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Prism Adaptation of Reaching Movements: Specificity for the Velocity of Reaching , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  P. Viviani,et al.  Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Carolyn R. Mason,et al.  Hand synergies during reach-to-grasp. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Adaptation of movement endpoints to perturbations of visual feedback , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[32]  P. Young,et al.  Time series analysis, forecasting and control , 1972, IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control.

[33]  J. Krakauer,et al.  A computational neuroanatomy for motor control , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  T. Ebner,et al.  Neuronal specification of direction and distance during reaching movements in the superior precentral premotor area and primary motor cortex of monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  F. Horak,et al.  Influence of globus pallidus on arm movements in monkeys. I. Effects of kainic acid-induced lesions. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  V. Franz,et al.  Manual size estimation: a neuropsychological measure of perception? , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Y Uno,et al.  Quantitative examinations of internal representations for arm trajectory planning: minimum commanded torque change model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  The basal ganglia network mediates the planning of movement amplitude , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[41]  Tunc Geveci,et al.  Advanced Calculus , 2014, Nature.

[42]  A. Riehle,et al.  Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  R. Held,et al.  PLASTICITY IN HUMAN SENSORIMOTOR CONTROL. , 1963, Science.

[44]  Charles E. Heckler,et al.  Applied Multivariate Statistical Analysis , 2005, Technometrics.

[45]  J Randall Flanagan,et al.  Visuomotor rotations of varying size and direction compete for a single internal model in motor working memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study , 1995, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  A. Barto Learning to reach via corrective movements , 1999 .

[48]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[49]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple joints. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  Petre Stoica,et al.  Decentralized Control , 2018, The Control Systems Handbook.

[51]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  宇野 洋二,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement : minimum torque-change model , 1988 .

[54]  B Wallace,et al.  Adaptive mechanisms in perceptual-motor coordination: components of prism adaptation. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[55]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Learning Optimal Adaptation Strategies in Unpredictable Motor Tasks , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  A. Bahill,et al.  Determining ideal baseball bat weights using muscle force-velocity relationships , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[58]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Effects of Pointing Rate and Availability of Visual Feedback on Visual and Proprioceptive Components of Prism Adaptation. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[59]  Marco Santello,et al.  Patterns of Hand Motion during Grasping and the Influence of Sensory Guidance , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Coarticulation in Fluent Fingerspelling , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[62]  D. Noll,et al.  Bilateral basal ganglia activation associated with sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  S. Kinomura,et al.  A PET Study of Visuomotor Learning under Optical Rotation , 2000, NeuroImage.

[64]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Parieto-frontal coding of reaching: an integrated framework , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[65]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  M. Kawato,et al.  Random presentation enables subjects to adapt to two opposing forces on the hand , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[67]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Covariation of primate dorsal premotor cell activity with direction and amplitude during a memorized-delay reaching task. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[68]  James C. Houk,et al.  Cerebellar learning for control of a two-link arm in muscle space , 1997, Proceedings of International Conference on Robotics and Automation.

[69]  Lennart Ljung,et al.  System Identification: Theory for the User , 1987 .

[70]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[71]  Paolo Viviani,et al.  Altering the visuomotor gain , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[72]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Differential cortical and subcortical activations in learning rotations and gains for reaching: a PET study. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  Hiroshi Imamizu,et al.  Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.

[74]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  A computational model of four regions of the cerebellum based on feedback-error learning , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[75]  Paolo Viviani,et al.  Altering the visuomotor gain. Evidence that motor plans deal with vector quantities. , 2002, Experimental brain research.

[76]  M. Kawato,et al.  Modular organization of internal models of tools in the human cerebellum , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[77]  W T Thach,et al.  Basal ganglia motor control. III. Pallidal ablation: normal reaction time, muscle cocontraction, and slow movement. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[78]  Xiaolin Liu,et al.  Contributions of online visual feedback to the learning and generalization of novel finger coordination patterns. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[79]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Learning to move amid uncertainty. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[80]  O. Bock,et al.  Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[81]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[82]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[83]  Martin Lemay,et al.  Multiple frames of reference for pointing to a remembered target , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[84]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Structure learning in action , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[85]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[86]  D. M. MacKay,et al.  Source density analysis of scalp potentials during linguistic and non-linguistic processing of visual stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[87]  Yasmin L. Hashambhoy,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reach Errors , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[88]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Motor subcircuits mediating the control of movement extent and speed. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[89]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Toward a new theory of motor synergies. , 2007, Motor control.

[90]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Evidence for the Flexible Sensorimotor Strategies Predicted by Optimal Feedback Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[91]  Andrew G. Barto,et al.  Reinforcement learning , 1998 .

[92]  P. Viviani,et al.  Error parsing in visuomotor pointing reveals independent processing of amplitude and direction. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[93]  J. Houk,et al.  Cerebellar guidance of premotor network development and sensorimotor learning. , 1997, Learning & memory.