On the limits of top-down control of visual selection
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] G. Campana,et al. Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[2] J. Theeuwes,et al. Detecting the presence of a singleton involves focal attention , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[3] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[4] Hsin-I Liao,et al. On the generality of the contingent orienting hypothesis. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[5] J. Theeuwes,et al. Visual search for featural singletons: No top-down modulation, only bottom-up priming , 2006 .
[6] H. J. Muller,et al. Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] Jan Theeuwes,et al. The role of cueing in attentional capture , 2008 .
[8] Bradley S. Gibson,et al. The identity intrusion effect: Attentional capture or perceptual load? , 2008 .
[9] S. Luck,et al. Bridging the Gap between Monkey Neurophysiology and Human Perception: An Ambiguity Resolution Theory of Visual Selective Attention , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.
[10] J. Theeuwes. Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[11] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[12] C. Bundesen. A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.
[13] A. Treisman. Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[14] E. Van der Burg,et al. The role of spatial and nonspatial information in visual selection. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] H. Müller,et al. Distinct, but top-down modulable color and positional priming mechanisms in visual pop-out search , 2009, Psychological research.
[16] H. Müller,et al. Locus of dimension weighting: Preattentive or postselective? , 2006 .
[17] N. P. Bichot,et al. Priming in Macaque Frontal Cortex during Popout Visual Search: Feature-Based Facilitation and Location-Based Inhibition of Return , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[19] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[20] Diane M. Beck,et al. Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain , 2009, Vision Research.
[21] J. Theeuwes. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[22] J. Theeuwes. Perceptual Selectivity for Color and Form: On the Nature of the Interference Effect , 1996 .
[23] J. Theeuwes. Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] J. Theeuwes,et al. Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings II: Additivity is no evidence for filtering , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[25] L. Chelazzi,et al. Behavioral/systems/cognitive Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate , 2022 .
[26] J. Theeuwes. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection: Reply to commentaries , 2010 .
[27] Jan Theeuwes,et al. SEARCH FOR A CONJUNCTIVELY DEFINED TARGET CAN BE SELECTIVELY LIMITED TO A COLOR-DEFINED SUBSET OF ELEMENTS , 1995 .
[28] N. P. Bichot,et al. A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.
[29] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[30] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] Eric Ruthruff,et al. Attentional capture with rapidly changing attentional control settings. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] Jan Theeuwes,et al. What is top-down about contingent capture? , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[33] J. Wolfe,et al. Changing your mind: on the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] Clayton Hickey,et al. Priming resolves perceptual ambiguity in visual search: Evidence from behaviour and electrophysiology , 2010, Vision Research.
[35] J. Theeuwes,et al. Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] R. Remington,et al. Additivity of abrupt onset effects supports nonspatial distraction, not the capture of spatial attention , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[37] J. Theeuwes,et al. Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[39] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Target uncertainty does not lead to more distraction by singletons: Intertrial priming does , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[40] DeLiang Wang,et al. The role of priming in conjunctive visual search , 2002, Cognition.