Spatial and Temporal Adaptations That Accompany Increasing Catching Performance During Learning

The authors studied changes in performance and kinematics during the acquisition of a 1-handed catch. Participants were 8 women who took an intensive 2-week training program during which they evolved from poor catchers to subexpert catchers. An increased temporal consistency, shift in spatial location of ball-hand contact away from the body, and higher peak velocity of the transport of the hand toward the ball accompanied their improvement in catching performance. Moreover, novice catchers first adjusted spatial characteristics of the catch to the task constraints and fine-tuned temporal features only later during learning. A principal components analysis on a large set of kinematic variables indicated that a successful catch depends on (a) forward displacement of the hand and (b) the dynamics of the hand closure, thereby providing a kinematic underpinning for the traditional transport-manipulation dissociation in the grasping and catching literature.

[1]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Temporal constraints in the control of prehensile movement. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[2]  Simon K. Rushton,et al.  Weighted combination of size and disparity: a computational model for timing a ball catch , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Relative Phase Alterations During Bimanual Skill Acquisition. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  G. J. K. Alderson,et al.  An operational analysis of a one-handed catching task using high speed photography. , 1974, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  H. T. A. Whiting,et al.  Specificity of motor learning does not deny flexibility , 1995 .

[6]  Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al.  Multiple information sources in interceptive timing , 1997 .

[7]  Keith Davids,et al.  Discriminating the role of binocular information in the timing of a one-handed catch , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[9]  Steven M. Finbeiner,et al.  The Neural and Behavioral Organization of Goal-Directed Movements , 1989, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[10]  Gilles Montagne,et al.  Reorganization of catching coordination under varying temporal constraints. , 2006, Motor control.

[11]  S. Bennett,et al.  Structural Organization of Practice: Effects of Practicing Under Different Informational Constraints on the Acquisition of One-Handed Catching Skill. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  R. Magill Motor Learning And Control , 1980 .

[13]  S. Glover,et al.  Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  Rob Gray,et al.  Different strategies for using motion-in-depth information in catching. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Gilles Montagne,et al.  The contribution of stereo vision to one-handed catching , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[17]  K. Newell Motor skill acquisition. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  Keith Davids,et al.  Acquiring coordination in self paced extrinsic timing tasks: a constraints led perspective , 1999 .

[19]  Ronald G. Marteniuk,et al.  Human Movement Organization and Learning As Revealed by Variability of Movement, use of Kinematic Information, and Fourier Analysis , 1983 .

[20]  G J Savelsbergh,et al.  The acquisition of catching under monocular and binocular conditions. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  A H FUCHS The progression-regression hypotheses in perceptual-motor skill learning. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[23]  M M Smyth,et al.  Vision and proprioception in simple catching. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  G J Savelsbergh,et al.  The effect of skill level, external frame of reference and environmental changes on one-handed catching. , 1988, Ergonomics.

[25]  D Elliott,et al.  I Lost It in the Lights: The Effects of Predictable and Variable Intermittent Vision on Unimanual Catching. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  S. Coleman,et al.  Examining Movement Variability in the Basketball Free-Throw Action at Different Skill Levels , 2003, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[27]  A. Gentile,et al.  Skill acquisition : Action, movement, and neuromotor processes , 1987 .

[28]  Simone R. Caljouw,et al.  The impact of task-constraints on the planning and control of interceptive hitting movements , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[29]  S. Bennett,et al.  Manipulating visual informational constraints during practice enhances the acquisition of catching skill in children. , 1999, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[30]  Nicola Brace,et al.  SPSS for psychologists: a guide to data analysis using SPSS for Windows (versions 9, 10 and 11). 2nd edition , 2007 .

[31]  M G Fischman,et al.  Skill level, vision, and proprioception in simple one-hand catching. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  A. Gentile,et al.  Form and Variability During Sit-to-Stand Transitions: Children Versus Adults , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  M. Jeannerod The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  R. Magill Motor learning and control : concepts and applications , 2004 .

[35]  M. Turvey,et al.  Temporal patterning in cascade juggling. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  J. Cooke,et al.  Changes in the variability of movement trajectories with practice. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  G. Montagne,et al.  The control and coordination of one-handed catching: the effect of temporal constraints , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Timing structure in squash strokes: Further evidence for the operational timing hypothesis , 1988 .