Interference of irrelevant information in multisensory selection depends on attentional set
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] C. Spence,et al. Multisensory distractor processing is modulated by spatial attention. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] S. Soto-Faraco,et al. Multisensory enhancement of attention depends on whether you are already paying attention , 2019, Cognition.
[3] C. Spence,et al. Overt Spatial Attention Modulates Multisensory Selection , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Patrick Chun Kau Chu,et al. Conflict monitoring in multi-sensory flanker tasks: Effects of cross-modal distractors on the N2 component , 2018, Neuroscience Letters.
[5] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Statistical Regularities Modulate Attentional Capture , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] C. Spence,et al. Crossmodal attentional control sets between vision and audition. , 2017, Acta psychologica.
[7] Zhe Qu,et al. The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture. , 2016, Psychophysiology.
[8] J. Kawahara,et al. Contingent attentional capture across multiple feature dimensions in a temporal search task. , 2016, Acta psychologica.
[9] C. Spence,et al. Multisensory top-down sets: Evidence for contingent crossmodal capture , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[10] Christian Frings,et al. The impact of the irrelevant: the task environment modulates the impact of irrelevant features in response selection. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] B. Hommel,et al. Attentional control of the creation and retrieval of stimulus–response bindings , 2014, Psychological research.
[12] C. Spence,et al. Response interference in touch, vision, and crossmodally: beyond the spatial dimension , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] C. Spence,et al. When vision influences the invisible distractor: tactile response compatibility effects require vision. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] Martin Eimer,et al. Top-down control of audiovisual search by bimodal search templates. , 2013, Psychophysiology.
[15] Y. Wada. Crossmodal Contextual Effects of Tactile Flankers on the Detection of Visual Targets , 2011 .
[16] Martin Eimer,et al. Multisensory enhancement of attentional capture in visual search , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[17] C. Spence. Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[18] D. Senkowski,et al. The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[19] N. Lavie. Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load , 2010 .
[20] C. Spence. Crossmodal spatial attention , 2010, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[21] Salvador Soto-Faraco,et al. Assessing the role of attention in the audiovisual integration of speech , 2010, Inf. Fusion.
[22] Charles Spence,et al. Capturing spatial attention with multisensory cues: A review , 2009, Hearing Research.
[23] Charles Spence,et al. Response Requirements Modulate Tactile Spatial Congruency Evects for Example, given That the Body Can Adopt a Variety of Diverent Postures, a Tactile Sensation Arising , 2022 .
[24] Charles Spence,et al. Capturing spatial attention with multisensory cues , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[25] Charles Spence,et al. Multisensory cues capture spatial attention regardless of perceptual load. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] R. A. Bailey. Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: a Model Comparison Perspective, 2nd edn , 2005 .
[27] Jason S. Chan,et al. Auditory spatial attention assessed in a flanker interference task , 2005 .
[28] N. Lavie. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[29] Charles L. Folk,et al. A Dissociation Between Attention and Selection , 2001, Psychological science.
[30] P Bertelson,et al. Directing spatial attention towards the illusory location of a ventriloquized sound. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[31] C. Spence,et al. The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[32] C. Spence,et al. Attracting attention to the illusory location of a sound: reflexive crossmodal orienting and ventriloquism , 2000, Neuroreport.
[33] B. Gibson,et al. Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for displaywide visual features. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] N. Lavie. Visual feature integration and focused attention: Response competition from multiple distractor features , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[35] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. GPOWER: A general power analysis program , 1996 .
[36] James L. McClelland,et al. Information integration in perception and communication , 1996 .
[37] N. Lavie. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] J. Theeuwes. Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[39] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] Scott E. Maxwell,et al. Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: A Model Comparison Perspective , 1990 .
[41] M. Braga,et al. Exploratory Data Analysis , 2018, Encyclopedia of Social Network Analysis and Mining. 2nd Ed..
[42] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .