Determinants of repeated blood donation among new and experienced blood donors

BACKGROUND: The maintenance of a safe level of blood supply is provided by a small number of volunteers, and their retention is difficult. The aim of this study was to identify factors predicting repeated blood donation among experienced and new donors.

[1]  Paul Norman,et al.  The Theory of Planned Behaviour and exercise: Evidence for the moderating role of past behaviour , 2000 .

[2]  Charles Abraham,et al.  Acting on intentions: the role of anticipated regret. , 2003, The British journal of social psychology.

[3]  S. Glynn,et al.  Convenience, the bane of our existence, and other barriers to donating , 2006, Transfusion.

[4]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[5]  Eamonn Ferguson,et al.  Predicting future blood donor returns: past behavior, intentions, and observer effects. , 2002, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[6]  J. Piliavin,et al.  Giving time, money, and blood: Similarities and differences. , 1999 .

[7]  H C Triandis,et al.  Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. , 1980, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[8]  R. Bagozzi Attitudes, intentions, and behavior: A test of some key hypotheses. , 1981 .

[9]  L. Cronbach Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests , 1951 .

[10]  Mark Conner,et al.  Social Cognitive Determinants of Blood Donation , 2001 .

[11]  S. Glynn,et al.  First year donation patterns predict long‐term commitment for first‐time donors , 2005, Vox sanguinis.

[12]  E. Ferguson Conscientiousness, emotional stability, perceived control and the frequency, recency, rate and years of blood donor behaviour. , 2004, British journal of health psychology.

[13]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Toward a theory-based analysis of behavioral maintenance. , 2000, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[14]  E. Cairns,et al.  An application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour to blood donation: the importance of self-efficacy. , 2004, Health education research.

[15]  J van der Pligt,et al.  Anticipated affective reactions and prevention of AIDS. , 1995, The British journal of social psychology.

[16]  J. van der Pligt,et al.  Anticipated Affect and Behavioral Choice , 1996 .

[17]  D. Dillman,et al.  EFFECTS OF QUESTIONNAIRE LENGTH, RESPONDENT-FRIENDLY DESIGN, AND A DIFFICULT QUESTION ON RESPONSE RATES FOR OCCUPANT-ADDRESSED CENSUS MAIL SURVEYS , 1993 .

[18]  I. Sarason,et al.  A Social Learning Approach to Increasing Blood Donations , 1991 .

[19]  E. Ferguson Predictors of future behaviour: A review of the psychological literature on blood donation , 1996 .

[20]  G. Kok,et al.  The Theory of Planned Behavior: A Review of its Applications to Health-Related Behaviors , 1996, American journal of health promotion : AJHP.

[21]  Antony Stephen Reid Manstead,et al.  The role of moral norm in the attitude-behavior relationship , 2000 .

[22]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior , 1980 .

[23]  James Jaccard,et al.  Cross-cultural model testing: Toward a solution of the etic-emic dilemma. , 1976 .

[24]  P. Callero,et al.  Role identity and reasoned action in the prediction of repeated behavior , 1988 .

[25]  J. L. D. Luna del Castillo,et al.  How Some Attitudes, Beliefs and Motivations of Spanish Blood Donors Evolve over Time , 1998, Vox Sanguinis.

[26]  Richard P. Bagozzi,et al.  An investigation into the role of intentions as mediators of the attitude-behavior relationship , 1989 .

[27]  Joop van der Pligt,et al.  Expectancy-Value models of health behaviour: the role of salience and anticipated affect , 1998 .

[28]  James Jaccard,et al.  An informational approach to altruistic behavior. , 1976 .

[29]  P. Sheeran,et al.  Augmenting the Theory of Planned Behavior: Roles for Anticipated Regret and Descriptive Norms , 1999 .

[30]  Mark Conner,et al.  Bridging the intention-behaviour 'gap': the role of moral norm. , 2004, The British journal of social psychology.

[31]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychological review.

[32]  J. Fleiss,et al.  Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  L. Cronbach,et al.  Time-limit tests: Estimating their reliability and degree of speeding , 1951, Psychometrika.

[34]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[35]  Wendy Wood,et al.  Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. , 1998 .

[36]  Charles D. Barrett Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior , 1980 .

[37]  D. Parker,et al.  Extending the theory of planned behaviour: The role of personal norm , 1995 .

[38]  Carl A. Kallgren,et al.  A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. , 1990 .

[39]  P. Pelto,et al.  Anthropological Research: The Structure of Inquiry , 1970 .

[40]  E. Cairns,et al.  Blood donation and Ajzen's theory of planned behaviour: an examination of perceived behavioural control. , 1995, The British journal of social psychology.

[41]  R. Bagozzi A Field Investigation of Causal Relations among Cognitions, Affect, Intentions, and Behavior , 1982 .

[42]  G Godin,et al.  Factors explaining the intention to give blood among the general population , 2005, Vox sanguinis.