Information synergy: adding unambiguous quality information rescues social information use in ants
暂无分享,去创建一个
Tomer J. Czaczkes | John J. Beckwith | Anna-Lena Horsch | T. Czaczkes | John J. Beckwith | Anna-Lena Horsch
[1] T. Czaczkes,et al. Individual ant workers show self-control , 2017, Biology Letters.
[2] Mandyam V Srinivasan,et al. Floral scents induce recall of navigational and visual memories in honeybees , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[3] Ofer Feinerman,et al. Ant trophallactic networks: simultaneous measurement of interaction patterns and food dissemination , 2015, Scientific Reports.
[4] M. Lihoreau,et al. Commentary: Do Bees Play the Producer-Scrounger Game? , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[5] K. Laland,et al. The evolutionary basis of human social learning , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[6] J. Keynes. A Treatise on Probability. , 1923 .
[7] N. Franks,et al. Knowledgeable individuals lead collective decisions in ants , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[8] F. Ratnieks,et al. Decision making in ant foragers (Lasius niger) facing conflicting private and social information , 2011, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[9] E. Leadbeater,et al. A social insect perspective on the evolution of social learning mechanisms , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[10] Ofer Feinerman,et al. Individual crop loads provide local control for collective food intake in ant colonies , 2018, eLife.
[11] How memory and motivation modulate the responses to trail pheromones in three ant species , 2016, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[12] R. Mann,et al. Social and private information influence the decision making of Australian meat ants (Iridomyrmex purpureus) , 2018, Insectes Sociaux.
[13] Anna Dornhaus,et al. Who Are the "Lazy" Ants? The Function of Inactivity in Social Insects and a Possible Role of Constraint: Inactive Ants Are Corpulent and May Be Young and/or Selfish. , 2017, Integrative and comparative biology.
[14] Kevin N Laland,et al. Nine-spined sticklebacks exploit the most reliable source when public and private information conflict , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[15] Anna Dornhaus,et al. Workers ‘specialized’ on inactivity: Behavioral consistency of inactive workers and their role in task allocation , 2015, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[16] J. Verhaeghe,et al. Food recruitment inTetramorium impurum (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) , 1982, Insectes Sociaux.
[17] P. Howse,et al. Orientation in leaf-cutting ants (Formicidae: Attini) , 1987, Animal Behaviour.
[18] I. Coolen,et al. Trade‐Offs in the Adaptive Use of Social and Asocial Learning , 2005 .
[19] Christopher D. Chambers,et al. Redefine statistical significance , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[20] C. Grüter,et al. Informational conflicts created by the waggle dance , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[21] D. Mautz,et al. Der Kommunikationseffekt der Schwänzeltänze bei Apis mellifica carnica (Pollm.) , 1971, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.
[22] J. Deneubourg,et al. Memory and chemical communication in the orientation of two mass-recruiting ant species , 1993, Insectes Sociaux.
[23] Sarah R. Heilbronner,et al. Ambiguity Aversion in Rhesus Macaques , 2010, Front. Neurosci..
[24] T. Seeley,et al. Dancing bees tune both duration and rate of waggle-run production in relation to nectar-source profitability , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[25] Craig R. Fox,et al. Ambiguity Aversion and Comparative Ignorance , 1995 .
[26] J. Deneubourg,et al. Modulation of trail laying in the antLasius niger (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) and its role in the collective selection of a food source , 1993, Journal of Insect Behavior.
[27] R. Morse. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees , 1994 .
[28] Dong-Hwan Choe,et al. Pheromone communication in ants: a detailed analysis of concentration-dependent decisions in three species , 2014, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[29] I. Coolen,et al. Adaptive trade-offs in the use of social and personal information. , 2009 .
[30] K. Laland,et al. Social learning strategies and predation risk: minnows copy only when using private information would be costly , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[31] W. Farina,et al. Trophallaxis: A Mechanism of Information Transfer , 2009 .
[32] F. Ratnieks,et al. Synergy between social and private information increases foraging efficiency in ants , 2011, Biology Letters.
[33] K. Laland. Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[34] James A. R. Marshall,et al. Decision-making under uncertainty: biases and Bayesians , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[35] J. Deneubourg,et al. How do ants assess food volume? , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[36] B. Webb,et al. Optimal cue integration in ants , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[37] J. Clobert,et al. Social information and emigration: lessons from immigrants. , 2007, Ecology letters.
[38] G. Dreisbach,et al. Voluntary Switching in an Invertebrate: The Effect of Cue and Reward Change , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[39] A. Dornhaus,et al. Foraging Bumble Bees Weigh the Reliability of Personal and Social Information , 2016, Current Biology.
[40] T. Valone,et al. Public Information: From Nosy Neighbors to Cultural Evolution , 2004, Science.
[41] F. Knight. The economic nature of the firm: From Risk, Uncertainty, and Profit , 2009 .
[42] I. Coolen,et al. Species difference in adaptive use of public information in sticklebacks , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[43] M. Gil,et al. Olfactory learning by means of trophallaxis in Apis mellifera , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[44] D. Ellsberg. Decision, probability, and utility: Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms , 1961 .
[45] H. Roche,et al. Why Copy Others? Insights from the Social Learning Strategies Tournament , 2010 .
[46] C. Grüter,et al. Insights from insects about adaptive social information use. , 2014, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[47] A. Tversky,et al. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .
[48] R. Rosengren,et al. Ortstreue in foraging ants of theFormica rufa group — Hierarchy of orienting cues and long-term memory , 1986, Insectes Sociaux.
[49] Y. Provecho,et al. Olfactory memory established during trophallaxis affects food search behaviour in ants , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[50] Roxana Josens,et al. Individual olfactory learning in Camponotus ants , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[51] T. Czaczkes,et al. Relative value perception in an insect: positive and negative incentive contrasts in ants , 2018, bioRxiv.
[52] I. Coolen,et al. Social Learning in Noncolonial Insects? , 2005, Current Biology.
[53] Thomas D. Seeley,et al. Honey bees use social information in waggle dances more fully when foraging errors are more costly , 2012 .
[54] Michael D. Breed,et al. Effects of experience on use of orientation cues in the giant tropical ant , 1989, Animal Behaviour.
[55] Yoram Halevy. Ellsberg Revisited: An Experimental Study , 2005 .
[56] Adam G. Hart,et al. Why do honey-bee (Apis millifera) foragers transfer nectar to several receivers? Information improvement through multiple sampling in a biological system , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[57] Christoph Grüter,et al. Social Learning: The Importance of Copying Others , 2010, Current Biology.
[58] Richard James,et al. Emergency networking: famine relief in ant colonies , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[59] Tomer J. Czaczkes,et al. Ants use directionless odour cues to recall odour-associated locations , 2014, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[60] Thierry Hoinville,et al. Steering intermediate courses: desert ants combine information from various navigational routines , 2016, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[61] Pheromone trail following in the ant Lasius niger: high accuracy and variability but no effect of task state , 2017 .
[62] Sasha R. X. Dall,et al. Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[63] V. Fourcassié,et al. How do red wood ants orient when foraging in a three dimensional system? I. Laboratory experiments , 1988, Insectes Sociaux.
[64] A. Tversky,et al. Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .
[65] J. Pasteels,et al. Spatial specialization of the foragers and foraging strategy inLasius fuliginosus (Latreille) (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) , 1996, Insectes Sociaux.
[66] Ellouise Leadbeater,et al. Foraging bumblebees do not rate social information above personal experience , 2014, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[67] D. Bates,et al. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.
[68] Tamsyn H. Bridger,et al. Ants show a leftward turning bias when exploring unknown nest sites , 2014, Biology Letters.
[69] B. Hare,et al. Chimpanzees and bonobos distinguish between risk and ambiguity , 2011, Biology Letters.
[70] Francis L. W. Ratnieks,et al. Social learning strategies in honeybee foragers: do the costs of using private information affect the use of social information? , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[71] T. Lillicrap,et al. Why Copy Others? Insights from the Social Learning Strategies Tournament , 2010, Science.
[72] J. Deneubourg,et al. Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions between scouts and nestmates in the ant Lasius niger , 2011, Insectes Sociaux.
[73] R. Josens,et al. Food information acquired socially overrides individual food assessment in ants , 2016, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[74] Chemical trail systems, orientation, and territorial interactions in the antLasius neoniger , 1989, Journal of Insect Behavior.