Reaching with cerebral tunnel vision

We studied reaching movements in a 48-year-old man with bilateral lesions of the calcarine cortex which spared the foveal representation and caused severe tunnel vision. Three-dimensional (3D) reconstruction of brain MR images showed no evidence of damage beyond area 18. The patient could not see his hand during reaching movements, providing a unique opportunity to test the role of peripheral visual cues in limb control. Optoelectronic recordings of upper limb movements showed normal hand paths and trajectories to fixated extrinsic targets. There was no slowing, tremor, or ataxia. Self-bound movements were also preserved. Analyses of limb orientation at the endpoints of reaches showed that the patient could transform an extrinsic target's visual coordinates to an appropriate upper limb configuration for target acquisition. There was no disadvantage created by blocking the view of the reaching arm. Moreover, the patient could not locate targets presented in the hemianopic fields by pointing. Thus, residual nonconscious vision or 'blindsight' in the aberrant fields was not a factor in our patient's reaching performance. The findings in this study show that peripheral visual cues on the position and velocity of the moving limb are not critical to the control of goal directed reaches, at least not until the hand is close to target. Other cues such as kinesthetic feedback can suffice. It also appears that the visuomotor transformations for reaching do not take place before area 19 in humans.

[1]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Visual effects of lesions of cortical area V2 in macaques , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  R F Hess,et al.  Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity in hemianopia. A comparative study of the sighted and blind hemifields. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Integration of visual information and motor output in reaching and grasping: The contributions of peripheral and central vision , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  M. Rizzo,et al.  Motion and shape perception in cerebral akinetopsia. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  M. Rizzo,et al.  The relative efficacy of cues for two-dimensional shape perception , 1996, Vision Research.

[7]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[8]  Monika Harvey,et al.  Balint's Patient , 1995 .

[9]  A. Georgopoulos Higher order motor control. , 1991, Annual review of neuroscience.

[10]  J F Soechting,et al.  Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 51–66 , 1909 .

[13]  M. Perenin,et al.  Visual function within the hemianopic field following early cerebral hemidecortication in man—I. Spatial localization , 1978, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  C. Bard,et al.  Directional control of rapid arm movements: the role of the kinetic visual feedback system. , 1993, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[16]  D. Roenker,et al.  Developmental changes in attention and visual search throughout adulthood. , 1990 .

[17]  D. Rotella,et al.  Troubled reaching after right occipito-temporal damage , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Utilization of Visual Feedback Information during Rapid Pointing Movements , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[19]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  M Corbetta,et al.  Effectiveness of different task paradigms in revealing blindsight. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  M. Rizzo 'Bálint's syndrome' and associated visuospatial disorders. , 1993, Bailliere's clinical neurology.

[22]  R Caminiti,et al.  Internal representations of movement in the cerebral cortex as revealed by the analysis of reaching. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Cerebral cortical mechanisms of reaching movements. , 1992, Science.

[24]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[25]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Factors affecting visual sensitivity in a hemianopic subject. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  K. Ball,et al.  Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. , 1993, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[30]  M. Perenin,et al.  Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  A. Damasio,et al.  Impairment of hand movements under visual guidance , 1979, Neurology.