The Influence of Age, Gender, and Cognitive Ability on the Susceptibility to Persuasive Strategies

The fact that individuals may react differently toward persuasive strategies gave birth to a shift in persuasive technology (PT) design from the one-size-fits-all traditional approach to the individualized approach which conforms to individuals’ preferences. Given that learners’ gender, age, and cognitive level can affect their response to different learning instructions, it is given primacy of place in persuasive educational technology (PET) design. However, the effect of gender, age, and cognitive ability on learners’ susceptibility to persuasive strategies did not receive the right attention in the extant literature. To close this gap, we carried out an empirical study among 461 participants to investigate whether learners’ gender, age, and cognitive ability significantly affect learners’ susceptibility to three key persuasive strategies (social learning, reward, and trustworthiness) in PETs. The results of a repeated measure analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) revealed that people with high cognitive level are more likely to be susceptible to social learning, while people with low cognitive level are more likely to be susceptible to trustworthiness. Comparatively, our results revealed that males are more likely to be susceptible to social learning, while females are more likely to be susceptible to reward and trustworthiness. Furthermore, our results revealed that younger adults are more likely to be susceptible to social learning and reward, while older adults are more likely to be susceptible to trustworthiness. Our findings reveal potential persuasive strategies which designers can employ to personalize PTs to individual users in higher learning based on their susceptibility profile determined by age, gender, and cognitive level.

[1]  Kathy Kellermann,et al.  Classifying Compliance Gaining Messages: Taxonomic Disorder and Strategic Confusion , 1994 .

[2]  Harri Oinas-Kukkonen,et al.  Persuasive Systems Design: Key Issues, Process Model, and System Features , 2009, Commun. Assoc. Inf. Syst..

[3]  Boris E. R. de Ruyter,et al.  Personalizing persuasive technologies: Explicit and implicit personalization using persuasion profiles , 2015, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[4]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Reward-Motivated Learning: Mesolimbic Activation Precedes Memory Formation , 2006, Neuron.

[5]  Michael Poulin,et al.  Growing to Trust , 2015 .

[6]  E. I. Sawin Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: The Classification of Educational Goals. Handbook 1. Committee of College and University Examiners, Benjamin S. Bloom , 1957 .

[7]  Miguel Nussbaum,et al.  Orchestration: Providing Teachers with Scaffolding to Address Curriculum Standards and Students. Pace of Learning , 2015, J. Educ. Technol. Soc..

[8]  Harri Oinas-Kukkonen,et al.  Influencing Individually: Fusing Personalization and Persuasion , 2012, TIIS.

[9]  Julia A. Leonard,et al.  Cognitive Skills, Student Achievement Tests, and Schools , 2014, Psychological science.

[10]  Julita Vassileva,et al.  Modeling the efficacy of persuasive strategies for different gamer types in serious games for health , 2014, User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction.

[11]  Lixia Yang,et al.  The Role of Cognition, Personality, and Trust in Fraud Victimization in Older Adults , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[12]  Maurice E. Schweitzer,et al.  Gender Differences in Trust Dynamics: Women Trust More than Men Following a Trust Violation , 2014 .

[13]  R. Cialdini,et al.  Social influence: Social norms, conformity and compliance. , 1998 .