On the advantage of being left-handed in volleyball: further evidence of the specificity of skilled visual perception

High ball speeds and close distances between competitors require athletes in interactive sports to correctly anticipate an opponent’s intentions in order to render appropriate reactions. Although it is considered crucial for successful performance, such skill appears impaired when athletes are confronted with a left-handed opponent, possibly because of athletes’ reduced perceptual familiarity with rarely encountered left-handed actions. To test this negative perceptual frequency effect hypothesis, we invited 18 skilled and 18 novice volleyball players to predict shot directions of left- and right-handed attacks in a video-based visual anticipation task. In accordance with our predictions, and with recent reports on laterality differences in visual perception, the outcome of left-handed actions was significantly less accurately predicted than the outcome of right-handed attacks. In addition, this left–right bias was most distinct when predictions had to be based on preimpact (i.e., before hand–ball contact) kinematic cues, and skilled players were generally more affected by the opponents’ handedness than were novices. The study’s findings corroborate the assumption that skilled visual perception is attuned to more frequently encountered actions.

[1]  A. Williams,et al.  Do pattern recognition skills transfer across sports? A preliminary analysis , 2004, Journal of sports sciences.

[2]  Charlotte A. Young,et al.  "Evolutionary" stable strategy of handedness in major league baseball. , 1996 .

[3]  James V. Stone,et al.  Object recognition: view-specificity and motion-specificity , 1999, Vision Research.

[4]  Greg Atkinson,et al.  Analysis of repeated measurements in physical therapy research: multiple comparisons amongst level means and multi-factorial designs , 2002 .

[5]  A. Williams,et al.  Perceptual expertise : development in sport , 2003 .

[6]  D. W. Holtzen Handedness and Professional Tennis , 2000, The International journal of neuroscience.

[7]  M. Gardner,et al.  Hand dominance influences the processing of observed bodies , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[8]  C. J. Wood,et al.  Handedness in 'fast ball' sports: do left-handers have an innate advantage? , 1989, British journal of psychology.

[9]  H. Heekeren,et al.  Provided for Non-commercial Research and Educational Use Only. Not for Reproduction, Distribution or Commercial Use. Advances in Coupling Perception and Action: the Quiet Eye as a Bidirectional Link between Gaze, Attention, and Action , 2022 .

[10]  T. McMorris,et al.  Anticipation of Professional Soccer Goalkeepers When Facing Right-and Left-Footed Penalty Kicks , 1996 .

[11]  B. Abernethy,et al.  How do World-Class Cricket Batsmen Anticipate a Bowler's Intention? , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  F. McKenna,et al.  Skilled anticipation in real-world tasks: measurement of attentional demands in the domain of tennis. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[13]  Florian Loffing,et al.  Automated processes in tennis: Do left-handed players benefit from the tactical preferences of their opponents? , 2010, Journal of sports sciences.

[14]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Priming and Recognition of Human Motion Patterns , 1997 .

[15]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Transfer of pattern recall skills may contribute to the development of sport expertise , 2005 .

[16]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Expertise and the Perception of Kinematic and Situational Probability Information , 2001, Perception.

[17]  K. Alexandris,et al.  Handedness in sport , 2002 .

[18]  Wolzt,et al.  World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. , 2003, The Journal of the American College of Dentists.

[19]  Derek T. Y. Mann,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: a meta-analysis. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[20]  C. Faurie,et al.  Handedness, homicide and negative frequency-dependent selection , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  James V. Stone Object recognition using spatiotemporal signatures , 1998, Vision Research.

[22]  J. Clarys,et al.  Science and Football II , 1993 .

[23]  D. Perrett,et al.  Integration of form and motion in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area (STPa) of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  Experience, context, and the visual perception of human movement. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Toward a general theory of expertise : prospects and limits , 1991 .

[26]  R. Ivry,et al.  Manual laterality and hitting performance in major league baseball. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Expert Performance in Sports: Advances in Research on Sport Expertise , 2003 .

[28]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Do Expertise and the Degree of Perception—Action Coupling Affect Natural Anticipatory Performance? , 2003, Perception.

[29]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Probing expert anticipation with the temporal occlusion paradigm: experimental investigations of some methodological issues. , 2005, Motor control.

[30]  Norbert Hagemann,et al.  The advantage of being left-handed in interactive sports , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[31]  K. Verfaillie,et al.  Viewpoint-dependent Priming Effects in the Perception of Human Actions and Body Postures , 1999 .

[32]  World Medical Association (WMA),et al.  Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects , 2009, Journal of the Indian Medical Association.

[33]  A. Williams,et al.  Recognizing and predicting movement effects: identifying critical movement features. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[34]  Dominique Pontier,et al.  Frequency-dependent maintenance of left handedness in humans , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[35]  D. Perrett,et al.  Evidence accumulation in cell populations responsive to faces: an account of generalisation of recognition without mental transformations , 1998, Cognition.

[36]  F. Allard,et al.  Perception in Sport: Basketball , 1980 .

[37]  K. Verfaillie Orientation-dependent priming effects in the perception of biological motion. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[40]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Visual analysis of body movements by neurones in the temporal cortex of the macaque monkey: A preliminary report , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[41]  K. Ericsson,et al.  Prospects and limits of the empirical study of expertise: an introduction , 1991 .

[42]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  J. Schorer,et al.  Relative Age Effects are a developmental problem in tennis: but not necessarily when you’re left‐handed! , 2010 .

[45]  John Hunt,et al.  Sinister strategies succeed at the cricket World Cup , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.