Sensory and hedonic judgments of common foods by lean consumers and consumers with obesity.

OBJECTIVE To establish differences between lean subjects and subjects with obesity in subjective reports of predominant taste and texture attributes of common foods, and the relationships of these qualities and foods with overall liking. RESEARCH METHODS AND PROCEDURES Twenty lean (body mass index: 20 to 25 kg/m2) nondieting healthy adults and 23 (body mass index: > or = 30 kg/m2) nondieting healthy adults with obesity assigned ratings for pleasantness and predominant sensory attributes to 50 common foods in 14 food groups using a questionnaire. They completed the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire. Body composition was assessed by bioelectrical impedance. RESULTS There were no significant group differences between pleasantness scores overall; however, lean subjects assigned higher scores to fruits and vegetables, to foods self-classified as sweet and as salty/savory, and to foods grouped in the highest and lowest quintiles of percentage food energy from fat. There were no group differences in free-choice texture descriptors applied to foods, but correspondence analysis revealed that subjects with obesity associated coarse, slippery, tough, and fatty textures with "dislike extremely," whereas lean subjects associated grainy, moist, doughy, and fibrous texture with "like extremely." Analyses reduced texture terms to 39 key descriptors for use in subsequent work. Groups did not significantly differ in reported external or restrained eating behaviors, but subjects with obesity scored significantly higher for reported emotional eating. DISCUSSION Differences in preferences for listed common foods exist between weight status groups according to taste, food group, and texture. Consumers can make textural descriptions of foods that could be used to assess sensory and hedonic qualities of actual food choices.

[1]  J. Ockene,et al.  Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. , 1995, International journal of epidemiology.

[2]  M. Carroll,et al.  Nutrient sources in the American diet: quantitative data from the NHANES II survey. II. Macronutrients and fats. , 1985, American journal of epidemiology.

[3]  T J Cole,et al.  Characteristics of the low-energy reporters in a longitudinal national dietary survey , 1997, British Journal of Nutrition.

[4]  Pascal Schlich,et al.  Correspondence analysis in sensory evaluation , 1991 .

[5]  L Lafay,et al.  Determinants and nature of dietary underreporting in a free-living population: the Fleurbaix Laventie Ville Santé (FLVS) study , 1997, International Journal of Obesity.

[6]  H. Kahn,et al.  Contrasting factors associated with abdominal and peripheral weight gain among adult women , 1997, International Journal of Obesity.

[7]  L. Birch,et al.  Fat preferences and fat consumption of 3- to 5-year-old children are related to parental adiposity. , 1995, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[8]  Adam Drewnowski,et al.  Food preferences in human obesity: Carbohydrates versus fats , 1992, Appetite.

[9]  K. Hunt,et al.  One apple a day?—fruit and vegetable intake in the West of Scotland , 1994 .

[10]  W. Willett,et al.  Is dietary fat a major determinant of body fat? , 1998, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[11]  Alina S. Szczesniak,et al.  MEANING OF TEXTURE WORDS TO THE CONSUMER , 1973 .

[12]  D. Mela,et al.  Take Five, a nutrition education intervention to increase fruit and vegetable intakes: impact on consumer choice and nutrient intakes , 1998, British Journal of Nutrition.

[13]  P. Rogers,et al.  Food, Eating and Obesity , 1998 .

[14]  R. Ortega,et al.  Estimated energy balance in female university students: differences with respect to body mass index and concern about body weight. , 1996, International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity.

[15]  R. Mensink,et al.  Underestimation of energy intake by 3-d records compared with energy intake to maintain body weight in 269 nonobese adults. , 1994, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[16]  D. Mela Eating behaviour, food preferences and dietary intake in relation to obesity and body-weight status , 1996, Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.

[17]  L. Lissner,et al.  Dietary fat and obesity: evidence from epidemiology. , 1995, European journal of clinical nutrition.

[18]  D. Schoeller Limitations in the assessment of dietary energy intake by self-report. , 1995, Metabolism: clinical and experimental.

[19]  D. Mela,et al.  Honest but invalid: what subjects say about recording their food intake. , 1997, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.

[20]  Adam Drewnowski,et al.  Sweet tooth reconsidered: Taste responsiveness in human obesity , 1985, Physiology & Behavior.

[21]  S Yoshikawa,et al.  COLLECTION AND CLASSIFICATION OF WORDS FOR DESCRIPTION OF FOOD TEXTURE: II: Texture Profiles. , 1970, Journal of texture studies.

[22]  Tatjana van Strien,et al.  The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior , 1986 .

[23]  J. Gower Generalized procrustes analysis , 1975 .

[24]  D. Mela,et al.  Sensory preferences for fats: relationships with diet and body composition. , 1991, The American journal of clinical nutrition.