"Schema Abstraction" in a Multiple-Trace Memory Model

A simulation model of episodic memory, MINERVA 2, is applied to the learning of concepts, as represented by the schema-abstraction task. The model assumes that each experience produces a separate memory trace and that knowledge of abstract concepts is derived from the pool of episodic traces at the time of retrieval. A retrieval cue contacts all traces simultaneously, activating each according to its similarity to the cue, and the information retrieved from memory reflects the summed content of all activated traces responding in parallel. The MINERVA 2 model is able to retrieve an abstracted prototype of the category when cued with the category name and to retrieve and disambiguate a category name when cued with a category exemplar. The model successfully predicts basic findings from the schema-abstraction literature (e.g., differential forgetting of prototypes and old instances, typicality, and category size effects), including some that have been cited as evidence against exemplar theories of concepts. The model is compared to other classification models, and its implications regarding the abstraction problem are discussed. How is abstract knowledge related to specific experience? In present-day terms, this question concerns the relationship between episodic and generic memories. This article explores the possibility that there is only one memory system, which stores episodic traces, and that abstract knowledge as such does not have to be stored but can be derived from the pool of traces of specific experiences at the time of retrieval. I demonstrate how this might work by applying a simulation model of a multipletrace memory theory to the sehema-abstraction experimental paradigm, which is widely believed to capture in the laboratory the processes by which generic or abstract ideas are formed. Multiple-trace theories assume that each event to which one attends gives rise to its own memory trace. Thus, repetition of an item such as a word in a list does not strengthen a prior representation (i.e., one predating the experiment or one laid down by the item's first experimental occurrence); rather, it produces a new trace that coexists in memory with traces of other occurrences of the same item. Experiments supporting the multiple-trace assumption have been primarily concerned with the ability of subjects to remember an item's presentation frequency, list membership, presentation modality, exposure duration, serial position, and so forth (e.g., Hintzman, 1976; Hintzman & Block, 1971; Hintzman, Block, & Summers, 1973;

[1]  E. Rosch,et al.  Structural bases of typicality effects. , 1976 .

[2]  John J. L. Morton,et al.  Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .

[3]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Stranger Behind the Engram: Theories of Memory and the Psychology of Science , 1982 .

[4]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[5]  F. Restle Psychology of judgment and choice , 1961 .

[6]  Anderson,et al.  LEVv The Effects of Category Generalizations and Instance Similarity on Schema Abstraction , 2022 .

[7]  J.A. Anderson,et al.  Theory of categorization based on distributed memory storage. , 1984 .

[8]  D. Robbins,et al.  The genesis and use of exemplar vs. prototype knowledge in abstract category learning , 1978 .

[9]  G. Bower,et al.  Storage and later recognition of exemplars of concepts , 1973 .

[10]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  Differential forgetting of prototypes and old instances: Simulation by an exemplar-based classification model , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[11]  J. Omohundro Recognition vs. classification of ill-defined category exemplars , 1981 .

[12]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Memory for frequency: A comparison of two multiple-trace theories. , 1981 .

[13]  Stephen A. Ritz,et al.  Distinctive features, categorical perception, and probability learning: some applications of a neural model , 1977 .

[14]  Ray Pike,et al.  Comparison of convolution and matrix distributed memory systems for associative recall and recognition , 1984 .

[15]  Emilie M. Roth,et al.  The effect of context on the structure of categories , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Accretion, tuning and restructuring: Three modes of learning , 1976 .

[17]  D. Medin,et al.  Linear separability in classification learning. , 1981 .

[18]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[19]  Margaret Jean Peterson,et al.  The abstraction and generalization of dot patterns , 1973 .

[20]  D. Homa,et al.  Integrative and metric properties of abstracted information as a function of category discriminability, instance variability, and experience. , 1977 .

[21]  E. Rosch,et al.  Cognition and Categorization , 1980 .

[22]  P. G. Neumann An attribute frequency model for the abstraction of prototypes , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[23]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  On putting apples into bottles — A problem of polysemy , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[24]  L. Jacoby Remembering the data: analyzing interactive processes in reading , 1983 .

[25]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  A MULTICOMPONENT THEORY OF THE MEMORY TRACE , 1977 .

[26]  F. Galton Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development , 1883 .

[27]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Modality tags and memory for repetitions: Locus of the spacing effect , 1973 .

[29]  D. Homa,et al.  Category Breadth and the Abstraction of Prototypical Information. , 1976 .

[30]  L. Brooks,et al.  Nonanalytic Cognition: Memory, Perception, and Concept Learning , 1984 .

[31]  J. Eich A composite holographic associative recall model. , 1982 .

[32]  D. L. Hintzman Repetition and Memory1 , 1976 .

[33]  D. Homa,et al.  Role of feedback, category size, and stimulus distortion on the acquisition and utilization of ill-defined categories , 1984 .

[34]  F. Hayes-Roth,et al.  Concept learning and the recognition and classification of exemplars , 1977 .

[35]  Donald Homa,et al.  Abstraction of stylistic concepts. , 1981 .

[36]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[37]  Mary C. Potter,et al.  Understanding noun phrases , 1979 .

[38]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Semantic factors in cognition , 1978 .

[39]  M. Posner,et al.  Retention of Abstract Ideas. , 1970 .

[40]  A. Tversky Features of Similarity , 1977 .

[41]  M. Posner,et al.  On the genesis of abstract ideas. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  D. Homa,et al.  Limitations of exemplar-based generalization and the abstraction of categorical information. , 1981 .

[43]  E. Tulving,et al.  Richard Semon's Theory of Memory , 1978 .

[44]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Repetition and memory: Evidence for a multiple-trace hypothesis. , 1971 .

[45]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory , 1984 .

[46]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Frequency as a nonpropositional attribute of memory. , 1982 .

[47]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[48]  L. Jacoby Perceptual enhancement: persistent effects of an experience. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Is memory schematic , 1983 .

[50]  E. Tulving Elements of episodic memory , 1983 .

[51]  D. Homa Prototype abstraction and classification of new instances as a function of number of instances defining the prototype , 1973 .

[52]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[53]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[54]  J. Jenkins,et al.  Abstraction over time of prototypes from distortions of random dot patterns: A replication , 1970 .

[55]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Categories and concepts , 1984 .