PII: S0042-6989(98)00082-0

Recent neurological studies of selective impairments in first and second-order motion processing are of considerable relevance in elucidating the mechanisms of motion perception in normal human observers. We examine the stimuli which have been used to assess first and second-order motion processing capabilities in clinical subjects, and discuss the nature of the computations necessary to extract their motion. We find that a simple computational model of first and second-order motion processing is able to account for the data. The model consists of a first-order channel computing motion at coarse and fine scales, and a coarse scale second-order channel. The second-order channel is sensitive to motion information defined by variations in luminance, contrast, spatial frequency and flicker. When elements of the model are disabled, its performance on either first or second-order motion can be selectively impaired in line with the neurological data. © 1998 Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

[1]  O. Braddick A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.

[2]  D. Pollen,et al.  Phase relationships between adjacent simple cells in the visual cortex. , 1981, Science.

[3]  C. Enroth-Cugell,et al.  Chapter 9 Visual adaptation and retinal gain controls , 1984 .

[4]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[5]  A. Derrington,et al.  Separate detectors for simple and complex grating patterns? , 1985, Vision Research.

[6]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[7]  Marc Green,et al.  What determines correspondence strength in apparent motion? , 1986, Vision Research.

[8]  H. Rodman,et al.  Coding of visual stimulus velocity in area MT of the macaque , 1987, Vision Research.

[9]  D J Heeger,et al.  Model for the extraction of image flow. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[10]  G. Sperling,et al.  Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[11]  A. Pantle,et al.  On the mechanism that encodes the movement of contrast variations: Velocity discrimination , 1989, Vision Research.

[12]  M. Landy,et al.  Kinetic depth effect and optic flow—I. 3D shape from Fourier motion , 1989, Vision Research.

[13]  G Sperling,et al.  Two motion perception mechanisms revealed through distance-driven reversal of apparent motion. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  Mary M. Conte,et al.  Motion mechanisms have only limited access to form information , 1990, Vision Research.

[15]  Michael S. Landy,et al.  The kinetic depth effect and optic flowȁII. First- and second-order motion , 1991, Vision Research.

[16]  G. Sperling,et al.  Texture quilts: Basic tools for studying motion-from-texture , 1991 .

[17]  K. Turano Evidence for a Common Motion Mechanism of Luminance-Modulated and Contrast-Modulated Patterns: Selective Adaptation , 1991, Perception.

[18]  C. Baker,et al.  Motion detection is dependent on spatial frequency not size , 1991, Vision Research.

[19]  G. Mather First-order and second-order visual processes in the perception of motion and tilt , 1991, Vision Research.

[20]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.

[21]  T. Albright,et al.  Motion coherency rules are form-cue invariant , 1992, Vision Research.

[22]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  A. Pantle Immobility of some second-order stimuli in human peripheral vision. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[24]  P. McOwan,et al.  A computational model of the analysis of some first-order and second-order motion patterns by simple and complex cells , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  H. Wilson,et al.  A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[26]  T D Albright,et al.  Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. , 1992, Science.

[27]  Mary M. Conte,et al.  Coherence and transparency of moving plaids composed of Fourier and non-Fourier gratings , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  S. Nishida,et al.  Positive motion after-effect induced by bandpass-filtered random-dot kinematograms , 1992, Vision Research.

[29]  Johannes M. Zanker,et al.  Theta motion: a paradoxical stimulus to explore higher order motion extraction , 1993, Vision Research.

[30]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Impaired visual motion perception in the contralateral hemifield following unilateral posterior cerebral lesions in humans. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  G. Mather,et al.  Evidence for second-order motion detectors , 1993, Vision Research.

[32]  S. Nishida Spatiotemporal properties of motion perception for random-check contrast modulations , 1993, Vision Research.

[33]  C. Baker,et al.  Different parameters control motion perception above and below a critical density , 1993, Vision Research.

[34]  C L Baker,et al.  A processing stream in mammalian visual cortex neurons for non-Fourier responses. , 1993, Science.

[35]  K. Nakayama,et al.  The characteristics of residual motion perception in the hemifield contralateral to lateral occipital lesions in humans. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  G. Sperling,et al.  The dimensionality of texture-defined motion: a single channel theory , 1993, Vision Research.

[37]  James E. McCarthy Directional adaptation effects with contrast modulated stimuli , 1993, Vision Research.

[38]  A. Derrington,et al.  Discriminating the direction of second-order motion at short stimulus durations , 1993, Vision Research.

[39]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Evidence for separate motion-detecting mechanisms for first- and second-order motion in human vision , 1994, Vision Research.

[40]  T. Ledgeway Adaptation to second-order motion results in a motion aftereffect for directionally-ambiguous test stimuli , 1994, Vision Research.

[41]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Direction identification thresholds for second-order motion in central and peripheral vision. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[42]  S. Cropper Velocity discrimination in chromatic gratings and beats , 1994, Vision Research.

[43]  G. Sperling,et al.  Full-wave and half-wave rectification in second-order motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[44]  G. Sperling,et al.  Perception of apparent motion between Dissimilar Gratings: Spatiotemporal properties , 1994, Vision Research.

[45]  S. Nishida,et al.  Complete interocular transfer of motion aftereffect with flickering test , 1994, Vision Research.

[46]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Correspondence-based and energy-based detection of second-order motion in human vision. , 1994, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[47]  David J. Fleet,et al.  Recursive Filters for Optical Flow , 1995, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..

[48]  D. Badcock,et al.  Global motion perception: No interaction between the first- and second-order motion pathways , 1995, Vision Research.

[49]  A. Gorea Spatiotemporal characterization of a Fourier and non-Fourier motion system , 1995, Vision Research.

[50]  G. Mather,et al.  Motion detection in interleaved random dot patterns: Evidence for a rectifying nonlinearity preceding motion analysis , 1995, Vision Research.

[51]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[52]  A. Johnston,et al.  A unified account of three apparent motion illusions , 1995, Vision Research.

[53]  A. Johnston,et al.  Perceived motion of contrast-modulated gratings: Predictions of the multi-channel gradient model and the role of full-wave rectification , 1995, Vision Research.

[54]  G. Sperling,et al.  The functional architecture of human visual motion perception , 1995, Vision Research.

[55]  J. T. Petersik A comparison of varieties of “second-order” motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[56]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  The perceived speed of second-order motion and its dependence on stimulus contrast , 1995, Vision Research.

[57]  S. Nishida,et al.  Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.

[58]  S. McKee,et al.  Temporal coherence theory for the detection and measurement of visual motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[59]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  Perceived visual speed constrained by image segmentation , 1996, Nature.

[60]  A. Cowey,et al.  Impairment of the perception of second order motion but not first order motion in a patient with unilateral focal brain damage , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[61]  J. Zanker On the elementary mechanism underlying secondary motion processing. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[62]  M. Hawken,et al.  Perceived velocity of luminance, chromatic and non-fourier stimuli: Influence of contrast and temporal frequency , 1996, Vision Research.

[63]  THOMAS V. PAPATHOMAS,et al.  Precise Assessment of the Mean Effective Luminance of Texture Patches—An Approach Based on Reverse-phi Motion , 1996, Vision Research.

[64]  M. Ibbotson,et al.  An adaptive Reichardt detector model of motion adaptation in insects and mammals , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[65]  G. Orban,et al.  The kinetic occipital region in human visual cortex. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[66]  J. Anthony Movshon,et al.  Neuronal responses to first- and second-order motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the alert macaque , 1997 .

[67]  S. Nishida,et al.  Simultaneous motion contrast across space: Involvement of second-order motion? , 1997, Vision Research.

[68]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Representation of motion boundaries in retinotopic human visual cortical areas , 1997, Nature.

[69]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Changes in perceived speed following adaptation to first-order and second-order motion , 1997, Vision Research.

[70]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Detection and Discrimination of First- and Second-Order Motion in Patients with Unilateral Brain Damage , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  P. McOwan,et al.  Perception of motion direction in luminance-and contrast-defined reversed-phi motion sequences , 1997, Vision Research.

[72]  A. Johnston,et al.  Speed discrimination thresholds for first- and second-order bars and edges , 1997, Vision Research.

[73]  ANDREW T SMITH,et al.  Separate Detection of Moving Luminance and Contrast Modulations: Fact or Artifact? , 1997, Vision Research.

[74]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in macaque area V3. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[75]  A. Cowey,et al.  The selective impairment of the perception of first-order motion by unilateral cortical brain damage , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.