If Attitudes Affect How Stimuli Are Processed, Should They Not Affect the Event-Related Brain Potential?

In Experiment 1, subjects completed an attitude survey to identify items toward which they held positive and negative attitudes. Subsequently, subjects were instructed to count the number of positive (or negative) stimuli in a series. Each series contained six attitude stimuli from a given semantic category (e.g., fruits), and the structure of the series was varied so that positive and negative stimuli, as indexed by subjects' idiosyncratic attitudes, were evaluatively consistent or inconsistent within the series. In Experiment 2, subjects were exposed to personality traits that were positive or negative in series of six. Again, the structure of the series was varied so that positive and negative traits were evaluatively consistent or inconsistent within the series. Results of Experiments 1 and 2 indicated that although the event-related brain potential did not differ as a function of stimulus valence per se, evaluatively inconsistent, in contrast to consistent, stimuli evoked a larger amplitude late P300-like positive component that was maximal over the centroparietal region.

[1]  R. Johnson,et al.  On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[2]  L. L. Thurstone,et al.  The measurement of social attitudes. , 1931 .

[3]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  Definition, Identification, and Reliability of Measurement of the P300 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential , 1987 .

[4]  E Donchin,et al.  The truth will out: interrogative polygraphy ("lie detection") with event-related brain potentials. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[5]  N. Squires,et al.  The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.

[6]  S. Cook,et al.  A MULTIPLE-INDICATOR APPROACH TO ATTITUDE MEASUREMENT. , 1964, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  W. Weiss MASS MEDIA AND SOCIAL CHANGE , 1970 .

[8]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  H. Lasswell,et al.  Propaganda and Communication in World History , 1982, Télos.

[10]  N. Anderson Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  Bruno Debruille,et al.  Brain potentials reveal covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[13]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[14]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  The handbook of social psychology, 2nd ed. , 1968 .

[15]  M. Taylor,et al.  Event-related potentials to visual and language stimuli in normal and dyslexic children. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[16]  M. Zanna,et al.  Physiological Mediation of Attitude Maintenance, Formation, and Change , 1984 .

[17]  Barbara A. Gutek,et al.  On the Accuracy of Retrospective Attitudinal Data , 1978 .

[18]  R. Hare,et al.  Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[19]  J. Rempel,et al.  Attitudes: A new look at an old concept. , 1988 .

[20]  Stephen E. Fienberg,et al.  Cognitive psychology meets the national survey. , 1985 .

[21]  E. Miller Handbook of Social Psychology , 1946, Mental Health.

[22]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Inferring psychological significance from physiological signals. , 1990, The American psychologist.

[23]  R. Tourangeau,et al.  Cognitive Processes Underlying Context Effects in Attitude Measurement , 1988 .

[24]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Neurobehavioral Organization and the Cardinal Principle of Evaluative Bivalence a , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[25]  I. Martin,et al.  Classical conditioning of human 'evaluative' responses. , 1975, Behaviour research and therapy.