The Effects of Individual and Context on Aggression in Repeated Social Interaction

In two studies using variations of the Prisoner's Dilemma game, we explore the impact of individual traits and social context on aggressive behavior. In the first study, we compared defection rates in the Iterated Prisoner's Dilemma when participants were presented with a payoff matrix (Description condition) or learned payoffs through experience (Experience condition). Interpersonal trust and maximizing tendency led to relatively less defection in the Description condition than in the Experience condition, demonstrating that individual characteristics manifest differently depending on the information available to decision-makers. In the second study, we employed a new game paradigm, the Intergroup Prisoner's Dilemma with Intragroup Power Dynamics, to examine the way that power motives influence extreme aggressive behavior. We discovered that certain individuals exhibit very high levels of defection, but only when they play with particular combinations of predefined strategies, further suggesting how the confluence of individual factors and context can induce aggression.

[1]  Martha Crenshaw,et al.  Decisions to use terrorism: Psychological constraints on instrumental reasoning: Martha Crenshaw , 2013 .

[2]  J R Wilson,et al.  Virtual environments applications and applied ergonomics. , 1999, Applied ergonomics.

[3]  A. Rapoport,et al.  Prisoner's Dilemma: A Study in Conflict and Co-operation , 1970 .

[4]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[5]  M. Nowak,et al.  A strategy of win-stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the Prisoner's Dilemma game , 1993, Nature.

[6]  K. Dodge,et al.  Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood: Relations to peer status and social context dimensions , 1989, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[7]  Robert Laird,et al.  Multidimensional latent-construct analysis of children's social information processing patterns: correlations with aggressive behavior problems. , 2002, Psychological assessment.

[8]  Guy H. Walker,et al.  A formative approach to developing synthetic environment fidelity requirements for decision-making training. , 2011, Applied ergonomics.

[9]  C. Insko,et al.  War and Peace: Possible Approaches to Reducing Intergroup Conflict , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[10]  R. Axelrod Effective Choice in the Prisoner's Dilemma , 1980 .

[11]  F. E. Grubbs Procedures for Detecting Outlying Observations in Samples , 1969 .

[12]  R. Putnam Diplomacy and domestic politics: the logic of two-level games , 1988, International Organization.

[13]  V. A. Harris,et al.  The Attribution of Attitudes , 1967 .

[14]  Ion Juvina,et al.  Intergroup Prisoner's Dilemma with Intragroup Power Dynamics and Individual Power Drive , 2011, SBP.

[15]  I. Erev,et al.  Small feedback‐based decisions and their limited correspondence to description‐based decisions , 2003 .

[16]  Nir Halevy,et al.  “In-Group Love” and “Out-Group Hate” as Motives for Individual Participation in Intergroup Conflict , 2008, Psychological science.

[17]  J. Nash Equilibrium Points in N-Person Games. , 1950, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  D. Hay,et al.  Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[19]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[20]  Scott Highhouse,et al.  Are maximizers really unhappy? The measurement of maximizing tendency , 2008, Judgment and Decision Making.

[21]  G. Bornstein,et al.  Perceiving Intergroup Conflict: From Game Models to Mental Templates , 2006, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[22]  L. Rowell Huesmann,et al.  An information processing model for the development of aggression , 1988 .

[23]  Todd Sandler,et al.  Chapter 25 Terrorism: A Game-Theoretic Approach⁎ , 2007 .

[24]  M. Rabin Published by: American , 2022 .

[25]  T. Yamagishi The provision of a sanctioning system as a public good , 1986 .

[26]  N. Miller,et al.  Frustration and aggression , 1939 .

[27]  J. Neumann,et al.  Prisoner's Dilemma , 1993 .

[28]  David G. Rand,et al.  Winners don’t punish , 2008, Nature.

[29]  C. Lebiere,et al.  A description-experience gap in social interactions: : information about interdependence and its effects on cooperation , 2013 .

[30]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[31]  W T Singleton Ergonomics and community needs. , 1971, Applied ergonomics.

[32]  Arnold H. Buss The Psychology of Aggression , 1961 .

[33]  Lorena R. R. Gianotti,et al.  A Neural Marker of Costly Punishment Behavior , 2010, Psychological science.

[34]  L. R. Huesmann,et al.  Stability of Aggression Over Time and Generations. , 1984 .

[35]  David M. Kreps,et al.  Reputation and imperfect information , 1982 .

[36]  S. Atran Genesis of Suicide Terrorism , 2003, Science.

[37]  Anatol Rapoport,et al.  The 2x2 Game , 1976 .

[38]  E. Salas,et al.  Establishing the Boundaries of a Paradigm for Decision-Making Research , 1996, Hum. Factors.

[39]  M. Johannesson,et al.  Heritability of ultimatum game responder behavior , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[40]  J. Henrich,et al.  Costly Punishment Across Human Societies , 2006, Science.

[41]  M. Crenshaw The Causes of Terrorism , 1981 .

[42]  K. Dodge,et al.  Aggression and antisocial behavior. , 1998 .

[43]  C. Patterson,et al.  Childhood aggression and peer relations in the context of family and neighborhood factors. , 1995, Child development.