Dissociating effects of global SWS disruption and healthy aging on waking performance and daytime sleepiness.

STUDY OBJECTIVE To contrast the effects of slow wave sleep (SWS) disruption and age on daytime functioning. DESIGN Daytime functioning was contrasted in three age cohorts, across two parallel 4-night randomized groups (baseline, two nights of SWS disruption or control, recovery sleep). SETTING Sleep research laboratory. PARTICIPANTS 44 healthy young (20-30 y), 35 middle-aged (40-55 y), and 31 older (66-83 y) men and women. INTERVENTIONS Acoustic stimulation contingent on appearance of slow waves. MEASUREMENTS AND RESULTS Cognitive performance was assessed before sleep latency tests at five daily time-points. SWS disruption resulted in less positive affect, slower or impaired information processing and sustained attention, less precise motor control, and erroneous implementation, rather than inhibition, of well-practiced actions. These performance impairments had far smaller effect sizes than the increase in daytime sleepiness and differed from baseline to the same extent for each age group. At baseline, younger participants performed better than older participants across many cognitive domains, with largest effects on executive function, response time, sustained attention, and motor control. At baseline, the young were sleepier than other age groups. CONCLUSIONS SWS has been considered a potential mediator of age-related decline in performance, although the effects of SWS disruption on daytime functioning have not been quantified across different cognitive domains nor directly compared to age-related changes in performance. The data imply that two nights of SWS disruption primarily leads to an increase in sleepiness with minor effects on other aspects of daytime functioning, which are different from the substantial effects of age.

[1]  G. Gunzelmann,et al.  Deconstructing and reconstructing cognitive performance in sleep deprivation. , 2013, Sleep medicine reviews.

[2]  M. Walker,et al.  Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves, and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Emma L. Arbon,et al.  Effects of Partial and Acute Total Sleep Deprivation on Performance across Cognitive Domains, Individuals and Circadian Phase , 2012, PloS one.

[4]  Kristine A. Wilckens,et al.  Age-Related Decline in Controlled Retrieval: The Role of the PFC and Sleep , 2012, Neural plasticity.

[5]  Donald Hedeker,et al.  A Practical Guide to Calculating Cohen’s f2, a Measure of Local Effect Size, from PROC MIXED , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[6]  Jennifer J. Richler,et al.  Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  Evelyne Balteau,et al.  Effects of Light on Cognitive Brain Responses Depend on Circadian Phase and Sleep Homeostasis , 2011, Journal of biological rhythms.

[8]  R. Huber,et al.  EEG sleep slow-wave activity as a mirror of cortical maturation. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Maya Ringli,et al.  Mapping of Cortical Activity in the First Two Decades of Life: A High-Density Sleep Electroencephalogram Study , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  I. Deary,et al.  The neuroscience of human intelligence differences , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  J. Born,et al.  The memory function of sleep , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Dijk,et al.  Age-related reduction in daytime sleep propensity and nocturnal slow wave sleep. , 2010, Sleep.

[13]  R. Marioni,et al.  Age-associated cognitive decline. , 2009, British medical bulletin.

[14]  G. Tononi,et al.  Sleep-dependent improvement in visuomotor learning: a causal role for slow waves. , 2009, Sleep.

[15]  I. Deary,et al.  Age-related Changes in Memory and Fluid Reasoning in a Sample of Healthy Old People , 2009, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[16]  J. Panksepp,et al.  Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  Daniel Aeschbach,et al.  A Role for Non-Rapid-Eye-Movement Sleep Homeostasis in Perceptual Learning , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  G. Tononi,et al.  Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  G. Tononi,et al.  Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. , 2006, Sleep medicine reviews.

[20]  Mirjam Münch,et al.  The frontal predominance in human EEG delta activity after sleep loss decreases with age , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[21]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  P. Achermann,et al.  Mathematical models of sleep regulation. , 2003, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[23]  D. Dinges,et al.  The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. , 2003, Sleep.

[24]  Maria L. Thomas,et al.  Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose‐response study , 2003, Journal of sleep research.

[25]  E. F. Colecchia,et al.  Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. , 2003, American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology.

[26]  J. Horne,et al.  Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults--a model for healthy aging? , 2000, Sleep.

[27]  K. Hume,et al.  Good sleep — its timing and physiological sleep characteristics , 1997, Journal of sleep research.

[28]  J. Born,et al.  Effects of Early and Late Nocturnal Sleep on Declarative and Procedural Memory , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  D.G.M. Dijk,et al.  Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  D. Dijk,et al.  Effect of unilateral somatosensory stimulation prior to sleep on the sleep EEG in humans , 1994, Journal of sleep research.

[31]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Sleep restriction and SWS‐suppression: effects on daytime alertness and night‐time recovery , 1994, Journal of sleep research.

[32]  A. Karni,et al.  Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. , 1994, Science.

[33]  G. Kecklund,et al.  Relations between performance and subjective ratings of sleepiness during a night awake. , 1994, Sleep.

[34]  P. Schweitzer,et al.  Slow‐wave sleep deprivation and waking function , 1994, Journal of sleep research.

[35]  A A Borbély,et al.  Electroencephalogram power density and slow wave sleep as a function of prior waking and circadian phase. , 1990, Sleep.

[36]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[37]  M H Bonnet,et al.  Performance and sleepiness as a function of frequency and placement of sleep disruption. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[38]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[39]  H. Agnew,et al.  Effects of prolonged stage four and 1-REM sleep deprivation. EEG, task performance, and psychologic responses.SAM-TR-67-59. , 1967, [Technical report] SAM-TR. USAF School of Aerospace Medicine.

[40]  H. Agnew,et al.  Comparison of stage four and 1-rem sleep deprivation. , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[41]  H. Agnew,et al.  THE EFFECTS OF STAGE FOUR SLEEP DEPRIVATION. , 1964, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[42]  Y. D. van der Werf,et al.  Reduction of nocturnal slow-wave activity affects daytime vigilance lapses and memory encoding but not reaction time or implicit learning. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[43]  S. Hughes,et al.  The slow (<1 Hz) rhythm of non-REM sleep: a dialogue between three cardinal oscillators , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Ian J Deary,et al.  Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of cognitive aging? , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[45]  P. Whitney,et al.  Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. , 2010, Sleep.

[46]  Denise C. Park,et al.  The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  M. Littner,et al.  Practice parameters for clinical use of the multiple sleep latency test and the maintenance of wakefulness test. , 2005, Sleep.

[48]  R. Rosa,et al.  The clinical use of the MSLT and MWT. , 2005, Sleep.

[49]  S. Daan,et al.  Quantitative analysis of the effects of slow wave sleep deprivation during the first 3 h of sleep on subsequent EEG power density , 2004, European archives of psychiatry and neurological sciences.

[50]  D. Dijk,et al.  Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. , 1999, Sleep research online : SRO.

[51]  M. Bertini,et al.  Selective slow-wave sleep (SWS) deprivation and SWS rebound: do we need a fixed SWS amount per night? , 1999, Sleep research online : SRO.

[52]  D. Sheehan,et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. , 1998, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[53]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .