What Determines Auditory Distraction? On the Roles of Local Auditory Changes and Expectation Violations

Both the acoustic variability of a distractor sequence and the degree to which it violates expectations are important determinants of auditory distraction. In four experiments we examined the relative contribution of local auditory changes on the one hand and expectation violations on the other hand in the disruption of serial recall by irrelevant sound. We present evidence for a greater disruption by auditory sequences ending in unexpected steady state distractor repetitions compared to auditory sequences with expected changing state endings even though the former contained fewer local changes. This effect was demonstrated with piano melodies (Experiment 1) and speech distractors (Experiment 2). Furthermore, it was replicated when the expectation violation occurred after the encoding of the target items (Experiment 3), indicating that the items' maintenance in short-term memory was disrupted by attentional capture and not their encoding. This seems to be primarily due to the violation of a model of the specific auditory distractor sequences because the effect vanishes and even reverses when the experiment provides no opportunity to build up a specific neural model about the distractor sequence (Experiment 4). Nevertheless, the violation of abstract long-term knowledge about auditory regularities seems to cause a small and transient capture effect: Disruption decreased markedly over the course of the experiments indicating that participants habituated to the unexpected distractor repetitions across trials. The overall pattern of results adds to the growing literature that the degree to which auditory distractors violate situation-specific expectations is a more important determinant of auditory distraction than the degree to which a distractor sequence contains local auditory changes.

[1]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Attention : selection, awareness, and control : a tribute to Donald Broadbent , 1996 .

[2]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  The mismatch negativity in cognitive and clinical neuroscience: Theoretical and methodological considerations , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[3]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  On the Irrelevance of Semantic Information for the “Irrelevant Speech” Effect , 1996 .

[4]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  The N1 hypothesis and irrelevant sound: evidence from token set size effects. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  D. C. Lecompte,et al.  An irrelevant speech effect with repeated and continuous background speech , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  D C LeCompte,et al.  Irrelevant speech and irrelevant tones: the relative importance of speech to the irrelevant speech effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  E. N. Solokov Perception and the conditioned reflex , 1963 .

[8]  I. Winkler,et al.  Memory prerequisites of mismatch negativity in the auditory event-related potential (ERP). , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  R. Knight,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Involuntary Attention to Acoustic Novelty and Change , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  E. Schröger,et al.  Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[11]  I. Winkler,et al.  Involuntary Attention and Distractibility as Evaluated with Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2000, Audiology and Neurotology.

[12]  Bill Macken,et al.  Role of Habituation in the Irrelevant Sound Effect : Evidence From the Effects of Token Set Size and Rate of Transition , 2001 .

[13]  Axel Buchner,et al.  Disruption of short-term memory by distractor speech: Does content matter? , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  Elke B. Lange Disruption of attention by irrelevant stimuli in serial recall , 2005 .

[15]  C. Escera,et al.  Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change , 2001, Neuroreport.

[16]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Processing of Abstract Rule Violations in Audition , 2007, PloS one.

[17]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory , and Cognition Broken Expectations : Violation of Expectancies , Not Novelty , Captures Auditory Attention , 2011 .

[18]  Axel Buchner,et al.  Irrelevant speech disrupts item-context binding. , 2013, Experimental psychology.

[19]  A. Buchner,et al.  The role of habituation and attentional orienting in the disruption of short-term memory performance , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[20]  Axel Buchner,et al.  Habituation of the irrelevant sound effect: evidence for an attentional theory of short-term memory disruption. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  Carles Escera,et al.  An electrophysiological and behavioral investigation of involuntary attention towards auditory frequency, duration and intensity changes. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Privileged Access by Irrelevant Speech to Short-term Memory: The Role of Changing State , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[23]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Objects, streams, and threads of auditory attention. , 1993 .

[24]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Auditory Distraction in Semantic Memory: A Process-Based Approach. , 2008 .

[25]  Alan B. Welsh,et al.  Acoustic masking in primary memory. , 1976 .

[26]  Carles Escera,et al.  The effect of age on involuntary capture of attention by irrelevant sounds: A test of the frontal hypothesis of aging , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Patrik Sörqvist,et al.  Expectations Modulate the Magnitude of Attentional Capture by Auditory Events , 2012, PloS one.

[28]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity and behavioural discrimination in humans as a function of the magnitude of change in sound duration , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[29]  Ian Neath The irrelevant sound effect is not always the same as the irrelevant speech effect , 2001 .

[30]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Auditory Distraction and Short-Term Memory: Phenomena and Practical Implications , 2001, Hum. Factors.

[31]  Axel Buchner,et al.  Valence of distractor words increases the effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[32]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  Negative priming and sequence learning , 2002 .

[33]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  Auditory memory and the irrelevant sound effect: Further evidence for changing-state disruption , 2002, Memory.

[34]  A. Buchner,et al.  Self-relevance increases the irrelevant sound effect: Attentional disruption by one's own name , 2013 .

[35]  E. Schröger,et al.  Early electrophysiological indicators for predictive processing in audition: a review. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[36]  Axel Buchner,et al.  Evidence for habituation of the irrelevant-sound effect on serial recall , 2014, Memory & cognition.

[37]  I. Winkler Interpreting the Mismatch Negativity , 2007 .

[38]  A. Buchner,et al.  Equivalent irrelevant-sound effects for old and young adults , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[39]  R. Näätänen,et al.  The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[40]  C. Escera,et al.  Role of Mismatch Negativity and Novelty-P3 in Involuntary Auditory Attention , 2007 .

[41]  R. Näätänen The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[42]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Auditory attentional capture during serial recall: violations at encoding of an algorithm-based neural model? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[44]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Regularity Extraction and Application in Dynamic Auditory Stimulus Sequences , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  L. Hasher,et al.  Automatic and effortful processes in memory. , 1979 .

[46]  Philip Beaman Irrelevant sound effects amongst younger and older adults: Objective findings and subjective insights , 2005 .

[47]  Erich Schrger,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Involuntary Attention Shifts to Changes in Auditory Stimulation , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  A. Buchner,et al.  ERP correlates of the irrelevant sound effect. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[49]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Disruption of short-term memory by changing and deviant sounds: support for a duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[50]  J. S. Nairne,et al.  The nature of remembering : essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder , 2001 .

[51]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[52]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Locus of the irrelevant speech effect in short-term memory , 1991 .

[53]  F. Barceló,et al.  Why are auditory novels distracting? Contrasting the roles of novelty, violation of expectation and stimulus change , 2011, Cognition.