Neural correlates of individual differences in spatial learning strategies.

Behavioral studies have shown that spatial skills, such as mental rotation, are correlated with preferences for certain types of spatial information. To be more specific, better mental rotation is associated with a preference for survey (maplike) spatial information relative to route (landmark or wayfinding) information. Functional MRI was used to investigate how individual differences in spatial skills (mental rotation) interact with encoding information from these 2 spatial perspectives. Despite similarities in performance across individuals for route and survey learning, differences between route and survey encoding activation increased with increased mental rotation ability in anterior cingulate, middle frontal gyrus, and postcentral gyrus. This correlation appeared to be due to decreasing activation during survey encoding and not activation changes during route learning. The results suggest that mental rotation skill contributes to survey or map learning but that alternative strategies can be used under the circumstances of this study to achieve equal performance.

[1]  Ian H. Robertson,et al.  Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies edited by Ian H. Robertson and John C. Marshall , 1994 .

[2]  G. Glover,et al.  Self‐navigated spiral fMRI: Interleaved versus single‐shot , 1998, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[3]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[4]  Stephen C. Hirtle,et al.  The acquisition of spatial knowledge , 1987 .

[5]  M. Denis,et al.  Spatial discourse and navigation: an analysis of route directions in the city of Venice , 1999 .

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Generalisability, Random Effects & Population Inference , 1998, NeuroImage.

[7]  M. Mesulam,et al.  From sensation to cognition. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  R. Passini Spatial representations, a wayfinding perspective , 1984 .

[9]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Mental spatial transformations of objects and perspective , 2001, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[10]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Association of Storage and Processing Functions in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of the Nonhuman Primate , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[12]  E. Rolls Spatial view cells and the representation of place in the primate hippocampus , 1999, Hippocampus.

[13]  R. Long,et al.  Predicting environmental learning from spatial abilities: An indirect route , 1996 .

[14]  D. Stuss,et al.  Principles of frontal lobe function , 2002 .

[15]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Encyclopedia of the Human Brain , 2002 .

[16]  R. Knight Distributed Cortical Network for Visual Attention , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  L. Kozlowski,et al.  Sense of Direction, Spatial Orientation, and Cognitive Maps. , 1977 .

[20]  A. Holmes Generalisability, random effects and population inference (abstract) , 1998 .

[21]  I. Silverman,et al.  The Hunter-Gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: Proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays , 1994 .

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[23]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  R. Parasuraman The attentive brain , 1998 .

[25]  Stephen C. Hirtle,et al.  Acquisition of spatial knowledge for routes , 1991 .

[26]  G. Micheletti The Prefrontal Cortex. Anatomy, Physiology and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, Fuster J.M.. Raven Press, New York (1989) , 1989 .

[27]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[28]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[29]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[31]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[32]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[33]  C. Lawton STRATEGIES FOR INDOOR WAYFINDING: THE ROLE OF ORIENTATION , 1996 .

[34]  J. Marshall,et al.  Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  R. De Beni,et al.  Strategies of processing spatial information in survey and landmark-centred individuals , 2001 .

[36]  I. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .

[37]  P. Goldman-Rakic Modular organization of prefrontal cortex , 1984, Trends in Neurosciences.

[38]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[39]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory , 1997, Nature.

[40]  Lynn A. Streeter,et al.  How to Tell People Where to Go: Comparing Navigational Aids , 1985, Int. J. Man Mach. Stud..

[41]  M. Posner,et al.  Executive attention: Conflict, target detection, and cognitive control. , 1998 .

[42]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Imagined transformations of bodies: an fMRI investigation , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  S. Bricogne,et al.  Neural Correlates of Topographic Mental Exploration: The Impact of Route versus Survey Perspective Learning , 2000, NeuroImage.

[44]  TIM R.H. CUTMORE,et al.  Cognitive and gender factors influencing navigation in a virtual environment , 2000, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[45]  D. Tucker,et al.  Electrophysiological Responses to Errors and Feedback in the Process of Action Regulation , 2003, Psychological science.

[46]  K. Jeffery,et al.  The Hippocampal and Parietal Foundations of Spatial Cognition , 1999 .

[47]  J. Money,et al.  Turner's syndrome: further demonstration of the presence of specific cognitional deficiencies. , 1966, Journal of medical genetics.

[48]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Distinguishing the Functional Roles of Multiple Regions in Distributed Neural Systems for Visual Working Memory , 2000, NeuroImage.

[49]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[50]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Neural Correlates of Encoding Space from Route and Survey Perspectives , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[52]  M. Hegarty,et al.  A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[53]  Shannon Dawn Moeser Cognitive Mapping in a Complex Building , 1988 .