Multivariate analysis of EEG activity indexes contingent and non-contingent attentional capture

It is well known that salient yet irrelevant singleton can capture attention, even when this is inconsistent with the current goals of the observer (Theeuwes, 1992; 2010). Others however have claimed that capture is critically contingent on the goals of the observer: Capture is strongly modulated (or even eliminated) when the irrelevant singleton does not match the target-defining properties (Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992). There has been a long-standing debate on whether attentional capture can be explained by goal-driven and/or stimulus-driven accounts. Here, we shed further light on this phenomenon by using EEG activity (raw EEG and alpha power) to provide a time-resolved index of attentional orienting. Participants searched for a target defined by a pre-specified color. The search display was preceded by a singleton cue that either matched the color of the upcoming target (contingent cues), or that appeared in an irrelevant color (non-contingent cues). Multivariate analysis of raw EEG and alpha power revealed preferential tuning to the location of both contingent and non-contingent cues, with a stronger bias towards contingent than non-contingent cues. The time course of these effects, however, depended on the neural signal. Raw EEG data revealed attentional orienting towards the cue early on in the trial (>156 ms), while alpha power revealed sustained spatial selection in the cued locations at a later moment in the trial (>250 ms). Moreover, while raw EEG showed stronger capture by contingent cues during this early time window, the advantage for contingent cues arose during a later time window in alpha band activity. Thus, our findings suggest that raw EEG activity and alpha-band power tap into distinct neural processes that index movements of covert spatial attention. Both signals provide clear neural evidence that both contingent and non-contingent cues can capture attention, and that this process is robustly shaped by the target-defining properties in the current block of trials.

[1]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  E. Vogel,et al.  Human Variation in Overriding Attentional Capture , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  M. Eimer The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  Joseph B. Hopfinger,et al.  Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[5]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Multivariate EEG analyses support high-resolution tracking of feature-based attentional selection , 2016, bioRxiv.

[6]  David M. Simpson,et al.  Induced activity in EEG in response to auditory stimulation , 2015, Biomed. Signal Process. Control..

[7]  R. Remington,et al.  Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture , 2006 .

[8]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  J. Theeuwes Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[11]  J. Serences,et al.  Near-Real-Time Feature-Selective Modulations in Human Cortex , 2013, Current Biology.

[12]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Involuntary Attentional Capture is Determined by Task Set: Evidence from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Christian N. L. Olivers,et al.  From ERPs to MVPA Using the Amsterdam Decoding and Modeling Toolbox (ADAM) , 2018, Front. Neurosci..

[14]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Attention selection, distractor suppression and N2pc , 2009, Cortex.

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Mechanisms of evoked and induced responses in MEG/EEG , 2006, NeuroImage.

[16]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Visual Selection: Usually Fast and Automatic; Seldom Slow and Volitional , 2018, Journal of cognition.

[17]  Juyang Weng,et al.  A computational developmental model for specificity and transfer in perceptual learning. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[18]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[19]  Christian N. L. Olivers,et al.  Rapid top-down control over template-guided attention shifts to multiple objects , 2017, NeuroImage.

[20]  J. Theeuwes Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings II: Additivity is no evidence for filtering , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[23]  D. Heeger,et al.  Decoding and Reconstructing Color from Responses in Human Visual Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Patryk A. Laurent,et al.  Value-driven attentional capture , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  David W. Sutterer,et al.  The topography of alpha-band activity tracks the content of spatial working memory. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  K. Cave,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .

[30]  B. Anderson A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[31]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Pavlovian reward learning underlies value driven attentional capture , 2016, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[32]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  David J. Hand,et al.  A Simple Generalisation of the Area Under the ROC Curve for Multiple Class Classification Problems , 2001, Machine Learning.

[36]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[38]  Gustavo Rohenkohl,et al.  Testing sensory evidence against mnemonic templates , 2015, eLife.

[39]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  What is top-down about contingent capture? , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[40]  Edward Awh,et al.  Alpha-Band Activity Reveals Spontaneous Representations of Spatial Position in Visual Working Memory , 2017, Current Biology.

[41]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Distinct roles of theta and alpha oscillations in the involuntary capture of goal-directed attention , 2017, NeuroImage.

[42]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[43]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Changing your mind: on the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  Hinze Hogendoorn,et al.  Perceptual integration without conscious access , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[47]  Edward Awh,et al.  Alpha-Band Oscillations Enable Spatially and Temporally Resolved Tracking of Covert Spatial Attention , 2017, Psychological science.

[48]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Decoding and Reconstructing the Focus of Spatial Attention from the Topography of Alpha-band Oscillations , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.