Preference for salt in a food may be alterable without a low sodium diet

Abstract In response to current efforts to reduce population-wide dietary salt intake, the objective of this study was to determine whether liking for reduced sodium and low sodium tomato juice could increase following repeated exposure over an extended period. Eighty-three adult subjects participated in a three-part study: an initial taste test, a 16-week longitudinal study, and a final taste test. Subjects gave liking ratings of four tomato juice samples ranging in sodium from 640 mg (a concentration comparable to a commercially available product) to 136 mg per 237 ml serving (a low sodium concentration) at both taste tests. For the longitudinal study, subjects were divided into two balanced groups based on PROP sensitivity, hedonic sensitivity to salt, and motivation to reduce dietary salt intake; the abrupt group received tomato juice reduced in sodium to reach a low sodium target at week four, and the gradual group received juice reduced in sodium via difference thresholds to reach the same target at week 14. Though liking for the juice with the highest salt content was unchanged between taste tests, liking for all reduced salt juices increased at the final taste test relative to the initial taste test among subjects in both salt reduction groups. In addition, subjects in both groups experienced a downward shift in preference for salt in tomato juice, indicating that repeated exposure may be sufficient to alter preference for salt in a food in the absence of a low sodium diet. That salt preference may be altered by exposure alone within the context of a high salt diet is promising for both the food industry and individual consumers.

[1]  Alessandro Braschi,et al.  Partial substitution of sodium with potassium in white bread: feasibility and bioavailability , 2009, International journal of food sciences and nutrition.

[2]  M. Gillette Flavor effects of sodium chloride , 1985 .

[3]  Linda M. Bartoshuk,et al.  Labeled scales (e.g., category, Likert, VAS) and invalid across-group comparisons: what we have learned from genetic variation in taste , 2003 .

[4]  Armand V. Cardello,et al.  A labeled affective magnitude (LAM) scale for assessing food liking/disliking , 2001 .

[5]  T. Thomas-Danguin,et al.  Using cross-modal interactions to counterbalance salt reduction in solid foods , 2011 .

[6]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Long-term reduction in dietary sodium alters the taste of salt. , 1982, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[7]  Christine L. Taylor,et al.  Strategies to Reduce Sodium Intake in the United States , 2010 .

[8]  M. Stieger,et al.  Saltiness enhancement by taste contrast in bread prepared with encapsulated salt , 2012 .

[9]  G. MacGregor,et al.  A comprehensive review on salt and health and current experience of worldwide salt reduction programmes , 2009, Journal of Human Hypertension.

[10]  Karen B DeSalvo,et al.  Dietary guidelines for Americans. , 2016, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[11]  N. Borhani,et al.  Effect of dietary sodium restriction on taste responses to sodium chloride: a longitudinal study. , 1986, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[12]  N. Bobowski A longitudinal comparison of two salt reduction strategies and acceptability of a low sodium food , 2013 .

[13]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Failure to compensate decreased dietary sodium with increased table salt usage. , 1987, JAMA.

[14]  Z. Vickers,et al.  Determining sequential difference thresholds for sodium chloride reduction , 2012 .

[15]  John Prescott,et al.  Changes in liking for a no added salt soup as a function of exposure , 2012 .

[16]  M. Stieger,et al.  Saltiness enhancement in bread by inhomogeneous spatial distribution of sodium chloride , 2010 .

[17]  R. Mattes,et al.  Relative contributions of dietary sodium sources. , 1991, Journal of the American College of Nutrition.

[18]  John E. Hayes,et al.  Explaining variability in sodium intake through oral sensory phenotype, salt sensation and liking , 2010, Physiology & Behavior.

[19]  Z. Vickers,et al.  Use of potassium chloride and flavor enhancers in low sodium Cheddar cheese. , 2013, Journal of dairy science.

[20]  Thierry Thomas-Danguin,et al.  Odour-taste interactions: a way to enhance saltiness in low-salt content solutions. , 2009 .

[21]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Increasing dietary salt alters salt taste preference , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[22]  Robert E. Weiss,et al.  Modeling Longitudinal Data , 2005 .

[23]  D. Chambers,et al.  A COMPARISON OF BASIC TASTE MODALITIES, USING A DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS TECHNIQUE, FOR VARYING LEVELS OF SODIUM AND KCl IN TWO MODEL SOUP SYSTEMS , 2010 .

[24]  P A Breslin,et al.  Suppression of bitterness by sodium: variation among bitter taste stimuli. , 1995, Chemical senses.

[25]  M. Woodward,et al.  A one-quarter reduction in the salt content of bread can be made without detection , 2003, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.