Participation in a behavioral weight-loss program worsens the prevalence and severity of underreporting among obese and overweight women.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Antonio H Lancha,et al. Selective underreporting of energy intake in women: magnitude, determinants, and effect of training. , 2003, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.
[2] M. McDowell,et al. Personality characteristics as predictors of underreporting of energy intake on 24-hour dietary recall interviews. , 2003, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.
[3] Rachel K. Johnson,et al. Impact of interviewer's body mass index on underreporting energy intake in overweight and obese women. , 2002, Obesity research.
[4] G. Johansson,et al. Underreporting of energy intake in repeated 24-hour recalls related to gender, age, weight status, day of interview, educational level, reported food intake, smoking habits and area of living , 2001, Public Health Nutrition.
[5] J. Lacour,et al. Simultaneous Validation of Ten Physical Activity Questionnaires in Older Men: A Doubly Labeled Water Study , 2001, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.
[6] T J Cole,et al. Biased over- or under-reporting is characteristic of individuals whether over time or by different assessment methods. , 2001, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.
[7] AE Black,et al. Critical evaluation of energy intake using the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake:basal metabolic rate. A practical guide to its calculation, use and limitations , 2000, International Journal of Obesity.
[8] A. Black. The sensitivity and specificity of the Goldberg cut-off for EI:BMR for identifying diet reports of poor validity , 2000, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.
[9] H. S. Bayley,et al. Four-day multimedia diet records underestimate energy needs in middle-aged and elderly women as determined by doubly-labeled water. , 2000, The Journal of nutrition.
[10] J E Blundell,et al. What foods do people habitually eat? A dilemma for nutrition, an enigma for psychology. , 2000, The American journal of clinical nutrition.
[11] C. Ritenbaugh,et al. The association of energy intake bias with psychological scores of women , 1999, European Journal of Clinical Nutrition.
[12] C. Drevon,et al. Under- and overreporting of energy intake related to weight status and lifestyle in a nationwide sample. , 1998, The American journal of clinical nutrition.
[13] J. Seidell,et al. Determinants of obesity-related underreporting of energy intake. , 1998, American journal of epidemiology.
[14] D. Kirschenbaum,et al. Further support for consistent self-monitoring as a vital component of successful weight control. , 1998, Obesity research.
[15] J. Ockene,et al. Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. , 1995, International journal of epidemiology.
[16] M. Goran,et al. Correlates of over- and underreporting of energy intake in healthy older men and women. , 1994, The American journal of clinical nutrition.
[17] C. Champagne,et al. Validity and reliability of reported dietary intake data. , 1994, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.
[18] S. Heymsfield,et al. Discrepancy between self-reported and actual caloric intake and exercise in obese subjects. , 1992, The New England journal of medicine.
[19] A. Gardner,et al. Resting energy metabolism and cardiovascular disease risk in resistance-trained and aerobically trained males. , 1992, Metabolism: clinical and experimental.
[20] S A Jebb,et al. Critical evaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 1. Derivation of cut-off limits to identify under-recording. , 1991, European journal of clinical nutrition.
[21] S A Jebb,et al. Critical evaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 2. Evaluating the results of published surveys. , 1991, European journal of clinical nutrition.
[22] J. Sallis,et al. Physical activity assessment methodology in the Five-City Project. , 1985, American journal of epidemiology.