Effects of two bitter substances on olfactory conditioning in the moth Heliothis virescens
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Demissew,et al. Phytochemical Dictionary: A Handbook of Bioactive Compounds from Plants , 1994, Economic Botany.
[2] M. Giurfa,et al. Aversive Learning in Honeybees Revealed by the Olfactory Conditioning of the Sting Extension Reflex , 2007, PloS one.
[3] D. Müller,et al. Anatomical organization of antennal lobe projection neurons in the moth Heliothis virescens , 2007, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[4] M. Mizunami,et al. Roles of octopaminergic and dopaminergic neurons in mediating reward and punishment signals in insect visual learning , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.
[5] G. Nagel,et al. Light-Induced Activation of Distinct Modulatory Neurons Triggers Appetitive or Aversive Learning in Drosophila Larvae , 2006, Current Biology.
[6] M. Zalucki,et al. Insect odour perception: recognition of odour components by flower foraging moths , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[7] H. Mustaparta,et al. Two closely located areas in the suboesophageal ganglion and the tritocerebrum receive projections of gustatory receptor neurons located on the antennae and the proboscis in the moth Heliothis virescens , 2006, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[8] H. Mustaparta,et al. A confined taste area in a lepidopteran brain. , 2006, Arthropod structure & development.
[9] H. Mustaparta,et al. Chapter Ten – Olfaction and Learning in Moths and Weevils Living on Angiosperm and Gymnosperm Hosts , 2005 .
[10] Martin Giurfa,et al. Electrophysiological and behavioural characterization of gustatory responses to antennal ‘bitter’ taste in honeybees , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[11] M. Mizunami,et al. Participation of octopaminergic reward system and dopaminergic punishment system in insect olfactory learning revealed by pharmacological study , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[12] N. Ryba,et al. The receptors and coding logic for bitter taste , 2005, Nature.
[13] 吉田 仁美,et al. Strength in Diversity , 2019, Bridging Communities through Socially Engaged Art.
[14] R Menzel,et al. Associative learning of plant odorants activating the same or different receptor neurones in the moth Heliothis virescens , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[15] E. Hartlieb. Olfactory conditioning in the mothHeliothis virescens , 1996, Naturwissenschaften.
[16] M. Heisenberg,et al. The carrot, not the stick: appetitive rather than aversive gustatory stimuli support associative olfactory learning in individually assayed Drosophila larvae , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[17] J. Hildebrand,et al. Learning modulates the ensemble representations for odors in primary olfactory networks. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[18] Kristin Scott,et al. Taste Representations in the Drosophila Brain , 2004, Cell.
[19] H. Amrein,et al. Taste Perception and Coding in Drosophila , 2004, Current Biology.
[20] Ronald L. Davis,et al. Altered Representation of the Spatial Code for Odors after Olfactory Classical Conditioning Memory Trace Formation by Synaptic Recruitment , 2004, Neuron.
[21] R. E. Page,et al. Sucrose responsiveness and behavioral plasticity in honey bees (Apis mellifera) , 2004 .
[22] Lars Chittka,et al. Fine colour discrimination requires differential conditioning in bumblebees , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.
[23] M. Heisenberg,et al. Visual learning in individually assayed Drosophila larvae , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[24] V. Taglietti,et al. Pulse coding in the gustative afferent fibres of the frog gustatory system , 2004, Pflügers Archiv.
[25] M. Heisenberg,et al. Dopamine and Octopamine Differentiate between Aversive and Appetitive Olfactory Memories in Drosophila , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] R. Menzel,et al. A new ascending sensory tract to the calyces of the honeybee mushroom body, the subesophageal‐calycal tract , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[27] R. Menzel,et al. Side-specific olfactory conditioning leads to more specific odor representation between sides but not within sides in the honeybee antennal lobes , 2003, Neuroscience.
[28] A. Dornhaus,et al. Psychophysics: Bees trade off foraging speed for accuracy , 2003, Nature.
[29] M. Mizunami,et al. Time Course of Protein Synthesis-Dependent Phase of Olfactory Memory in the Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus , 2003, Zoological science.
[30] Frederic Mery,et al. Experimental evolution of learning ability in fruit flies , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[31] M. Mizunami,et al. Lifetime olfactory memory in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus , 2002, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[32] R. Menzel,et al. Visualizing mushroom body response to a conditioned odor in honeybees , 2001, Naturwissenschaften.
[33] S. Chandra,et al. Quantitative Trait Loci Associated with Reversal Learning and Latent Inhibition in Honeybees (Apis mellifera) , 2001, Behavior genetics.
[34] R. Menzel,et al. Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[35] Jayaram Chandrashekar,et al. A Novel Family of Mammalian Taste Receptors , 2000, Cell.
[36] B. K. Mitchell,et al. Peripheral and central structures involved in insect gustation , 1999, Microscopy research and technique.
[37] B. Hansson,et al. Appetitive Learning of Odours With Different Behavioural Meaning in Moths , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.
[38] R. Menzel. Memory dynamics in the honeybee , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[39] J. Erber,et al. Tactile learning and the individual evaluation of the reward in honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[40] S. West,et al. Learning in Helicoverpa armigera (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae): a new look at the behaviour and control of a polyphagous pest , 1999 .
[41] D. Laloi,et al. Olfactory conditioning of the proboscis extension in bumble bees , 1999 .
[42] R. Menzel,et al. Associative learning modifies neural representations of odors in the insect brain , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] R. Menzel,et al. Long‐ but not medium‐term retention of olfactory memories in honeybees is impaired by actinomycin D and anisomycin , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.
[44] Anderson,et al. Behavioural analysis of olfactory conditioning in the moth spodoptera littoralis (Boisd.) (Lepidoptera: noctuidae) , 1997, The Journal of experimental biology.
[45] R. Lubow. Latent inhibition as a measure of learned inattention: some problems and solutions , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.
[46] T. Hills,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for two transduction pathways within a bitter-sensitive taste receptor. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] F. Marion-Poll,et al. Un‐filtered recordings from insect taste sensilla , 1996 .
[48] J. Glendinning. Is chemosensory input essential for the rapid rejection of toxic foods? , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.
[49] D Schild,et al. Second messenger signaling in olfactory transduction. , 1996, Journal of neurobiology.
[50] J. Glendinning,et al. Gustatory habituation to deterrent allelochemicals in a herbivore: concentration and compound specificity , 1995, Animal Behaviour.
[51] R. Margolskee,et al. A cyclicnucleotidesuppressible conductance activated by transducin in taste cells , 1995, Nature.
[52] J C Sandoz,et al. Olfactory learning and memory in the honeybee: comparison of different classical conditioning procedures of the proboscis extension response. , 1995, Comptes rendus de l'Academie des sciences. Serie III, Sciences de la vie.
[53] M. Hammer,et al. Learning and memory in the honeybee , 1995 .
[54] Quinine-HCI-Induced Modification of Receptor Potentials for Taste Stimuli in Frog Taste Cells , 1995, Zoological science.
[55] J. Glendinning,et al. Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.
[56] T. Préat,et al. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila , 1994, Cell.
[57] A. Dubin,et al. Transduction diversity in olfaction. , 1994, The Journal of experimental biology.
[58] B. Ache,et al. Towards a common strategy for transducing olfactory information. , 1994, Seminars in cell biology.
[59] G. Schultz,et al. Some taste substances are direct activators of G-proteins. , 1994, The Biochemical journal.
[60] E. Bernays,et al. Host-Plant Selection by Phytophagous Insects , 1994, Contemporary Topics in Entomology.
[61] M. Hammer. An identified neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybees , 1993, Nature.
[62] R. Menzel,et al. Inhibition of brain protein synthesis by cycloheximide does not affect formation of long-term memory in honeybees after olfactory conditioning , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[63] R. Menzel. Associative learning in honey bees , 1993 .
[64] E. Bernays,et al. Relationship between deterrence and toxicity of plant secondary compounds for the alfalfa weevil Hypera brunneipennis , 1992 .
[65] M. Bitterman,et al. Learning in Honeybees (Apis mellifera) As a Function of Sucrose Concentration , 1992 .
[66] S. Kinnamon. Role of K+ channels in taste transduction. , 1992, Society of General Physiologists series.
[67] S. Kinnamon,et al. Chemosensory transduction mechanisms in taste. , 1992, Annual review of physiology.
[68] J. Brand,et al. The diversity of bitter taste signal transduction mechanisms. , 1992, Society of General Physiologists series.
[69] R. Chapman,et al. SENSORY CODING FOR FEEDING DETERRENCE IN THE GRASSHOPPER SCHISTOCERCA AMERICANA , 1991 .
[70] R. Rouseff. Bitterness in foods and beverages , 1990 .
[71] M. Akabas,et al. A bitter substance induces a rise in intracellular calcium in a subpopulation of rat taste cells. , 1988, Science.
[72] M. Simmonds,et al. Food selection in adults and larvae of three species of Lepidoptera: a behavioural and electro physiological study , 1988 .
[73] I. Shimada. Stereospecificity of the multiple receptor sites in the sugar taste receptor cell of the fleshfly , 1987 .
[74] M. Bitterman,et al. Latent inhibition in honeybees , 1986 .
[75] M. Berenbaum,et al. Insect-Plant Interactions , 1986, Springer Series in Experimental Entomology.
[76] T. McGuire,et al. Conditioned suppression of proboscis extension in Drosophila melanogaster. , 1985, Journal of comparative psychology.
[77] J. Caprio,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for acidic, basic, and neutral amino acid olfactory receptor sites in the catfish , 1984, The Journal of general physiology.
[78] M. Bitterman,et al. Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). , 1983, Journal of comparative psychology.
[79] M. Frank,et al. Effects of binary taste stimuli on the neural activity of the hamster chorda tympani , 1980, The Journal of general physiology.
[80] R. A. Bell,et al. Techniques for Rearing Laboratory Colonies of Tobacco Hornworms and Pink Bollworms , 1976 .
[81] J. Erber,et al. Retrograde amnesia in honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica). , 1976, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[82] I. Shimada. Two receptor sites and their relation to amino acid stimulation in the labellar sugar receptor of the fleshfly. , 1975, Journal of insect physiology.
[83] K Kurihara,et al. Inhibition of cyclic 3′, 5′‐nucleotide phosphodiesterase in bovine taste papillae by bitter taste stimuli , 1972, FEBS letters.
[84] A. W. Herre. Sharks in Fresh Water , 1955, Science.
[85] K. D. Roeder,et al. Physiology of a primary chemoreceptor unit. , 1955, Science.