Association of the Distinct Visual Representations of Faces and Names: A PET Activation Study

A PET study of seven normal individuals was carried out to investigate the neural populations involved in the retrieval of the visual representation of a face when presented with an associated name, and conversely. Face-name associations were studied by means of four experimental matching conditions, including the retrieval of previously learned (1) name-name (NN), (2) face-face (FF), (3) name-face (NF), and (4) face-name (FN) associations, as well as a resting scan with eyes closed. Before PET images acquisition, subjects were presented with 24 unknown face-name associations to encode in 12 male/female couples. During PET scanning, their task was to decide whether the presented pair was a previously learned association. The right fusiform gyrus was strongly activated in FF condition as compared to NN and Rest conditions. However, no specific activations were found for NN condition relative to FF condition. A network of three areas distributed in the left hemisphere, both active in (NF-FF) and (FN-NN) comparisons, was interpreted as the locus of the integration of visual faces and names representations. These three regions were localized in the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45), the medial frontal gyrus (BA 6) and the supramarginal gyrus of the inferior parietal lobe (BA 40). An interactive model accounting for these results, with BA 40 seen as an amodal binding region, is proposed.

[1]  C J Price,et al.  The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  S. Petersen,et al.  Frontal cortex contributes to human memory formation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Verbal Working Memory Load Affects Regional Brain Activation as Measured by PET , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  N. Cohen,et al.  Hippocampal system and declarative (relational) memory: Summarizing the data from functional neuroimaging studies , 1999, Hippocampus.

[6]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Neural Systems Underlying the Recognition of Familiar and Newly Learned Faces , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Carlo Semenza,et al.  Generating proper names: A case of selective inability , 1988 .

[9]  R. Campbell,et al.  Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging of crossmodal binding in the human heteromodal cortex , 2000, Current Biology.

[10]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[11]  Walter Dandy,et al.  The Brain , 1966 .

[12]  M. Crommelinck,et al.  Effect of Familiarity on the Processing of Human Faces , 1999, NeuroImage.

[13]  T. Allison,et al.  Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  R. Dolan,et al.  Contrast polarity and face recognition in the human fusiform gyrus , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychology: Patterns of Co-occurrence Among the Associative Agnosias: Implications for Visual Object Representation , 1991 .

[16]  Theodor Landis,et al.  Are Unilateral Right Posterior Cerebral Lesions Sufficient to Cause Prosopagnosia? Clinical and Radiological Findings in Six Additional Patients , 1986, Cortex.

[17]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Segregating Semantic from Phonological Processes during Reading , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Marcia K. Johnson MEM: Mechanisms of Recollection , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  R. Morris,et al.  Neural correlates of planning ability: Frontal lobe activation during the tower of London test , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  N C Andreasen,et al.  Direct comparison of the neural substrates of recognition memory for words and faces. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Premotor and Prefrontal Correlates of Category-Related Lexical Retrieval , 1998, NeuroImage.

[22]  A. Young,et al.  Handbook of Research on Face Processing , 1989 .

[23]  E K Warrington,et al.  Testing for nominal dysphasia. , 1980, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[24]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  PET Evidence for an Amodal Verbal Working Memory System , 1996, NeuroImage.

[25]  J. Kessler,et al.  Differential impairments in recalling people's names: A case study in search of neuroanatomical correlates , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  T. Allison,et al.  Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Tarr,et al.  Activation of the middle fusiform 'face area' increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  T. Allison,et al.  Face-sensitive regions in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  J. Haxby,et al.  The distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Richard D. Hichwa,et al.  A neural basis for lexical retrieval , 1996, Nature.

[31]  N Kanwisher,et al.  THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF FACE PROCESSING: AN INTRODUCTION , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[32]  A. Damasio Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[33]  A. Roskies The Binding Problem , 1999, Neuron.

[34]  Costanza Papagno,et al.  Proper name anomia: A case with sparing of the first-letter knowledge , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  J. Sergent,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  E. Renzi,et al.  Prosopagnosia can be associated with damage confined to the right hemisphere—An MRI and PET study and a review of the literature , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Automated image registration: I. General methods and intrasubject, intramodality validation. , 1998, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[38]  H J Tochon-Danguy,et al.  Updating working memory for words: A PET activation study , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[39]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Hemispheric Asymmetries for Whole-Based and Part-Based Face Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex during tests of episodic memory , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  Janice Kay,et al.  Selective Impairment of the Retrieval of People's Names: A Case of Category Specificity , 1995, Cortex.

[42]  C D Frith,et al.  Brain activity during memory retrieval. The influence of imagery and semantic cueing. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  T. Hanakawa,et al.  Transient Neural Activity in the Medial Superior Frontal Gyrus and Precuneus Time Locked with Attention Shift between Object Features , 1999, NeuroImage.

[44]  M. Farah,et al.  Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  M. Tarr,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area is Part of a Network that Processes Faces at the Individual Level , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  The Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Recognition Memory and Memory for Source: An fMRI Study , 1999, NeuroImage.

[47]  P E Roland,et al.  Cross-Modal Transfer of Information between the Tactile and the Visual Representations in the Human Brain: A Positron Emission Tomographic Study , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  A. Damasio,et al.  Neural regionalization of knowledge access: preliminary evidence. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[49]  C. Semenza,et al.  Anomia for people's names , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  R. Carney,et al.  Prosopanomia? a possible category-specific anomia for faces , 1993 .

[52]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Binocular Rivalry and Visual Awareness in Human Extrastriate Cortex , 1998, Neuron.

[53]  E. Renzi,et al.  Proper Name Anomia , 1992, Cortex.

[54]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[55]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind. , 1996 .

[57]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Selective Letter Generation: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1998, Cortex.