Positive emotion broadens attention focus through decreased position-specific spatial encoding in early visual cortex: Evidence from ERPs
暂无分享,去创建一个
Gilles Pourtois | Valentina Rossi | Naomi Vanlessen | G. Pourtois | R. Raedt | V. Rossi | Naomi Vanlessen | Rudi Raedt
[1] B. Fredrickson. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. , 2001, The American psychologist.
[2] Ki-Young Jung,et al. Influence of task difficulty on the features of event-related potential during visual oddball task , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.
[3] E Donchin,et al. A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.
[4] G. Pourtois,et al. Top-down effects on early visual processing in humans: A predictive coding framework , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[5] A. Anderson,et al. Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[6] E. Rolls. The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[7] Greg L. West,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence for Biased Competition in V1 for Fear Expressions , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] Gilles Pourtois,et al. Time Course of Brain Activity during Change Blindness and Change Awareness: Performance is Predicted by Neural Events before Change Onset , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] N. Lavie. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] I.. THE ATTENTION SYSTEM OF THE HUMAN BRAIN , 2002 .
[11] E. Miller,et al. Response to Comment on "Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices" , 2007, Science.
[12] Arno Villringer,et al. A Physiological Correlate of the “Zoom Lens” of Visual Attention , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] M. Posner,et al. Selective attention and cognitive control , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.
[14] Christoph M. Michel,et al. Two electrophysiological stages of spatial orienting towards fearful faces: early temporo-parietal activation preceding gain control in extrastriate visual cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.
[15] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.
[16] Christoph M. Michel,et al. Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Human Cognition. , 1999, News in physiological sciences : an international journal of physiology produced jointly by the International Union of Physiological Sciences and the American Physiological Society.
[17] J. Wolfe,et al. What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[18] G. Band,et al. Reward Counteracts Conflict Adaptation , 2009, Psychological science.
[19] E. Rolls,et al. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology , 2004, Progress in Neurobiology.
[20] Christoph Mulert,et al. Dopaminergic mechanisms of target detection — P300 event related potential and striatal dopamine , 2011, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.
[21] E. Watkins,et al. Concreteness training reduces dysphoria: A pilot proof-of-principle study , 2009, Behaviour research and therapy.
[22] Karen Gasper,et al. Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global Versus Local Processing of Visual Information , 2002, Psychological science.
[23] D. Levine,et al. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. , 1999, Psychological review.
[24] D. Derryberry,et al. Temperament and attention: orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[25] U. Castiello,et al. Size of the attentional focus and efficiency of processing. , 1990, Acta psychologica.
[26] G. Dreisbach,et al. How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[27] M. Posner,et al. Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.
[28] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[29] Gilles Pourtois,et al. State-dependent attention modulation of human primary visual cortex: A high density ERP study , 2012, NeuroImage.
[30] Rui M. Costa,et al. Frontocerebellar Connectivity: Climbing through the Inferior Olive , 2010, Front. Neurosci..
[31] E. Viding,et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[32] Jeremy R. Gray,et al. Integration of Emotion and Cognitive Control , 2004 .
[33] G. Band,et al. In the Mood for Adaptation , 2010, Psychological science.
[34] D C Van Essen,et al. Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective. , 1992, Science.
[35] S. Luck,et al. Sources of attention-sensitive visual event-related potentials , 2005, Brain Topography.
[36] Robert Tibshirani,et al. Estimating the number of clusters in a data set via the gap statistic , 2000 .
[37] A. Isen,et al. Positive Affect Facilitates Integration of Information and Decreases Anchoring in Reasoning among Physicians , 1997 .
[38] J. M. Carroll,et al. On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.
[39] J. Henry,et al. The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. , 2004, The British journal of clinical psychology.
[40] S. Hillyard,et al. Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses , 1994 .
[41] J. Polich. Updating P 300 : An Integrative Theory of P 3 a and P 3 b , 2009 .
[42] Manuel Schabus,et al. Abnormal neural filtering of irrelevant visual information in depression , 2009, NeuroImage.
[43] Gilles Pourtois,et al. Transient state-dependent fluctuations in anxiety measured using STAI, POMS, PANAS or VAS: a comparative review , 2012, Anxiety, stress, and coping.
[44] R. Knight,et al. Mechanisms of human attention: event-related potentials and oscillations , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[45] A. Isen,et al. The influence of affect on categorization. , 1984 .
[46] Yu Wang,et al. The impact of emotion valence on brain processing of behavioral inhibitory control: Spatiotemporal dynamics , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.
[47] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[48] J. Vaidya,et al. The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence , 1999 .
[49] C. Eriksen,et al. Allocation of attention in the visual field. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[50] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[51] A. Isen,et al. Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[52] D. Watson,et al. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[53] R. Marois,et al. Capacity limits of information processing in the brain , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[54] A. Beck,et al. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. , 1996, Journal of personality assessment.
[55] G. Pourtois,et al. Attentional load modifies early activity in human primary visual cortex , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[56] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.
[57] Lynn Hasher,et al. Delighted and distracted: positive affect increases priming for irrelevant information. , 2011, Emotion.
[58] S. Hillyard,et al. Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[59] J. Polich,et al. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review , 1995, Biological Psychology.
[60] L. Hasher,et al. Positive mood is associated with the implicit use of distraction , 2010, Motivation and Emotion.
[61] Denis Brunet,et al. Topographic ERP Analyses: A Step-by-Step Tutorial Review , 2008, Brain Topography.
[62] Jeffrey S. Maxwell,et al. Stress Potentiates Early and Attenuates Late Stages of Visual Processing , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[63] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[64] Jonas K. Olofsson,et al. Is the P600/SPS affected by the richness of semantic content? A linguistic ERP study in Swedish. , 2008, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[65] S. Hillyard,et al. Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[66] L. Pessoa,et al. Embedding Reward Signals into Perception and Cognition , 2010, Front. Neurosci..
[67] E. Holmes,et al. Positive interpretation training: effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood. , 2006, Behavior therapy.
[68] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[69] D. Lehmann,et al. Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.
[70] A. Isen,et al. Positive affect and decision making. , 1993 .
[71] M. Carrasco. Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.
[72] Y. Tsal,et al. Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[73] A. Isen,et al. The Influence of Positive Affect on Clinical Problem solving , 1991, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.
[74] G. Bower,et al. In search of mood-dependent retrieval. , 1989 .
[75] U. Castiello,et al. Splitting focal attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[76] C. Carver,et al. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales , 1994 .
[77] J. Theeuwes. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[78] J. Gray,et al. Emotional modulation of cognitive control: approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[79] S. Yantis,et al. Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.
[80] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[81] Jun'ichi Katayama,et al. Difficulty of discrimination modulates attentional capture for deviant information. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[82] S. Delplanque,et al. Beyond Conventional Event-related Brain Potential (ERP): Exploring the Time-course of Visual Emotion Processing Using Topographic and Principal Component Analyses , 2008, Brain Topography.
[83] R. Ivry,et al. The two sides of perception , 1997 .
[84] J. G. Axford,et al. Source locations of pattern-specific components of human visual evoked potentials. II. Component of extrastriate cortical origin , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[85] L. F. Barrett,et al. Handbook of Emotions , 1993 .
[86] Steven C. R. Williams,et al. A functional MRI study of happy and sad affective states induced by classical music , 2007, Human brain mapping.
[87] L. F. Barrett,et al. Handbook of emotions, 2nd ed. , 2000 .
[88] A. Keil,et al. Modulation of the C1 visual event-related component by conditioned stimuli: evidence for sensory plasticity in early affective perception. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[89] D. Grandjean,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.
[90] D. Grandjean,et al. Enhanced extrastriate visual response to bandpass spatial frequency filtered fearful faces: Time course and topographic evoked‐potentials mapping , 2005, Human brain mapping.
[91] H. Moriya,et al. Effect of mood states on the breadth of spatial attentional focus: An event-related potential study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[92] R. Dolan,et al. Attentional load and sensory competition in human vision: modulation of fMRI responses by load at fixation during task-irrelevant stimulation in the peripheral visual field. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[93] Philip A. Gable,et al. The Blues Broaden, but the Nasty Narrows , 2010, Psychological science.
[94] C. Spielberger,et al. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .
[95] E. Holmes,et al. Looking at or through rose-tinted glasses? Imagery perspective and positive mood. , 2008, Emotion.
[96] N. Lavie. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[97] L. Chelazzi,et al. Behavioral/systems/cognitive Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate , 2022 .
[98] B. Fredrickson,et al. Positive Emotions Speed Recovery from the Cardiovascular Sequelae of Negative Emotions. , 1998, Cognition & emotion.
[99] Philip A. Gable,et al. Approach-Motivated Positive Affect Reduces Breadth of Attention , 2008, Psychological science.
[100] S. Kosslyn,et al. Intuitions and introspections about imagery: the role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator's theoretical views , 2003 .
[101] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.