Neurobiology of perceptual and motor timing in children with spina bifida in relation to cerebellar volume.

The cerebellum is important for perceptual and motor timing in the mature brain, but the timing function of the cerebellum in the immature brain is less well understood. We investigated timing in children with spina bifida meningomyelocele (SB), a neural tube defect that involves cerebellar dysgenesis, and in age-matched controls. Specifically, we studied perceptual timing (judgements of 400 ms duration) and motor timing (isochronous motor tapping); measured cerebellar volumes; and related perceptual and motor timing to each other and to cerebellar volume measurements. Children with SB had impairments in the perception of duration (around 400 ms) but not frequency (around 3000 Hz), showing that their perceptual timing deficit was not a generalized auditory impairment. Children with SB had motor timing deficits on unpaced but not paced isochronous tapping, and their unpaced timing performance was associated with clock variance rather than with motor implementation. Perceptual and motor timing were correlated, suggesting that children with SB have impairments in a central timing mechanism. Children with SB, especially those with upper spinal cord lesions, had significant cerebellar volume reductions in grey and white matter, as well as different regional patterns of grey matter, white matter and CSF. Duration perception was correlated with cerebellar volumes, and the number of valid tapping trials was correlated with cerebellar volumes in the SB group, which data demonstrate structure-function relations between timing and cerebellar volumes.

[1]  D. Mclone,et al.  The cause of Chiari II malformation: a unified theory. , 1989, Pediatric neuroscience.

[2]  Richard E Thompson,et al.  Cerebellar circuits and synaptic mechanisms involved in classical eyeblink conditioning , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  L. Jäncke,et al.  Cortical activations during paced finger-tapping applying visual and auditory pacing stimuli. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[4]  R Tannock,et al.  Time perception deficits in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder and comorbid reading difficulties in child and adolescent samples. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[5]  M. A. Merrill,et al.  Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale , 1972 .

[6]  V Bracha,et al.  Patients with cerebellar lesions cannot acquire but are able to retain conditioned eyeblink reflexes. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  M Dennis,et al.  Perception and estimation of time in long-term survivors of childhood posterior fossa tumors , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[8]  M. D. Del Bigio,et al.  Neuropathological changes caused by hydrocephalus. , 1993, Acta neuropathologica.

[9]  I. Levin,et al.  Time estimation by adults who stutter. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[10]  L. Hartlage Pediatric Neuropsychology Research, Theory and Practice , 2001 .

[11]  R B Ivry,et al.  Dissociable contributions of the prefrontal and neocerebellar cortex to time perception. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[12]  Massimo Pandolfo,et al.  The Cerebellum and its Disorders: SPORADIC DISEASES , 2001 .

[13]  S. Keele,et al.  Timing Functions of The Cerebellum , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Variance decomposition of tempo drift in isochronous rhythmic tapping. , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[15]  J. Fletcher,et al.  The changing nervous system : neurobehavioral consequences of early brain disorders , 1999 .

[16]  Richard B Ivry,et al.  Temporal Control and Coordination: The Multiple Timer Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[17]  M. Khoury,et al.  Is there etiologic heterogeneity between upper and lower neural tube defects? , 1992, American journal of epidemiology.

[18]  M. Mauk,et al.  Cerebellar function: Coordination, learning or timing? , 2000, Current Biology.

[19]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses , 1973 .

[20]  J. Binder,et al.  Distributed Neural Systems Underlying the Timing of Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Timing of behavior : neural, psychological, and computational perspectives , 1998 .

[22]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Motor Timing: A PET Study of Auditory and Visual Rhythm Reproduction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Perceptual timing in cerebellar degeneration , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  R. Ivry The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[25]  V. Albernaz Pediatric Neuroimaging, , 2nd ed., A. James Barkovich. Raven, New York (1994), 648 pp., hardcover, $145 , 1995 .

[26]  J A Fiez,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Cognition , 1996, Neuron.

[27]  K. Volcik,et al.  Examinations of methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase C677T and A1298C mutations--and in utero viability. , 2001, American journal of human genetics.

[28]  J. Emery,et al.  The Weight of the Cerebellum in Children with Myelomeningocele , 1973, Developmental medicine and child neurology. Supplement.

[29]  Steve G. Massaquoi,et al.  The Cerebellum and its Disorders: Models of cerebellar function , 2001 .

[30]  H. Blom,et al.  Folate, Homocysteine and Neural Tube Defects: An Overview , 2001, Experimental biology and medicine.

[31]  S. Mostofsky,et al.  Duration Judgments in Children With ADHD Suggest Deficient Utilization of Temporal Information Rather Than General Impairment in Timing , 2004, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[32]  R. Miall,et al.  Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: evidence from neuroimaging , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  G. B. Wetherill,et al.  SEQUENTIAL ESTIMATION OF POINTS ON A PSYCHOMETRIC FUNCTION. , 1965, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[34]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Increased variability of paced finger tapping accuracy following repetitive magnetic stimulation of the cerebellum in humans , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  R. Miall,et al.  Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  R. Clark,et al.  Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  S. Keele,et al.  Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  E. A. Franz,et al.  Evidence of Common Timing Processes in the Control of Manual, Orofacial, and Speech Movements. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[41]  Guido Gerig,et al.  Nonlinear anisotropic filtering of MRI data , 1992, IEEE Trans. Medical Imaging.

[42]  R. Barkley,et al.  Time perception and reproduction in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2001, Neuropsychology.

[43]  T. Rammsayer,et al.  Neuropharmacological Evidence for Different Timing Mechanisms in Humans , 1999, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[44]  M. D. Bigio,et al.  Neuropathological changes caused by hydrocephalus , 2004, Acta Neuropathologica.

[45]  S. Mostofsky,et al.  Judgment of Duration in Individuals With Ataxia-Telangiectasia , 2000, Developmental neuropsychology.

[46]  A. Paolitto,et al.  Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. , 2000 .

[47]  Yoh-Han Pao,et al.  Adaptive pattern recognition and neural networks , 1989 .

[48]  M. Dennis,et al.  Motor function profile in children with early onset hydrocephalus , 1999 .

[49]  R. Ivry,et al.  Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  Larry A. Kramer,et al.  Morphometric evaluation of the hydrocephalic brain: relationships with cognitive development , 1996, Child's Nervous System.

[51]  M. Pinsk,et al.  Functional Localization of a “Time Keeper” Function Separate from Attentional Resources and Task Strategy , 2000, NeuroImage.

[52]  N M Thompson,et al.  Verbal and nonverbal skill discrepancies in hydrocephalic children. , 1992, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[53]  M. Brandt,et al.  Estimation of CSF, white and gray matter volumes in hydrocephalic children using fuzzy clustering of MR images. , 1994, Computerized medical imaging and graphics : the official journal of the Computerized Medical Imaging Society.

[54]  S. Keele,et al.  Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: a correctional analysis. , 1985, Acta psychologica.

[55]  M. Dennis,et al.  Neuromotor Speech Deficits in Children and Adults with Spina Bifida and Hydrocephalus , 2002, Brain and Language.

[56]  J. Hore,et al.  Finger opening in an overarm throw is not triggered by proprioceptive feedback from elbow extension or wrist flexion , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  D. McLone The cause of Chiari 2 malformation , 1989 .

[58]  Reliability of brain structure morphometry in hydrocephalic children using MR images. , 1996, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[59]  J. Steinmetz Brain substrates of classical eyeblink conditioning: a highly localized but also distributed system , 2000, Behavioural Brain Research.