Our need for associative coherence

The world around us consists of typical settings manifested as statistical regularities and stored as associations. These associations are beneficial for performance, and we rely on them as a source of stability in our perception of a coherent surrounding. What happens when such associativity is not apparent? We presented pairs of associated images and pairs of non-associated images and compared their corresponding effect on subsequent performance in three different visual perception paradigms: contrast-sensitivity, global vs. local perception, and critical-flicker-fusion. In all three experiments, performance was significantly inferior when preceded by images with no clear associative connection. We propose that these results reflect the need to find coherence in our environment. When such coherence is not easily detected, we seem to persist in seeking for it, which in turn poses a lingering cognitive load that taxes performance even in low-level perception.

[1]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  J. Glover,et al.  Handbook of creativity. , 1989 .

[3]  M. Bar,et al.  Human preferences are biased towards associative information , 2015, Cognition & emotion.

[4]  I. Biederman Perceiving Real-World Scenes , 1972, Science.

[5]  M. Bar The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  I. Levin Creativity and two modes of associative fluency: Chains and stars1 , 1978 .

[8]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Further Evidence of One-Trial Associative Learning , 1959 .

[9]  Cognitive load eliminates the global perceptual bias for unlimited exposure durations , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Perceptual load modulates conscious flicker perception. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[11]  M. Bar Visual objects in context , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  E. Simonson,et al.  Flicker fusion frequency; background and applications. , 1952, Physiological reviews.

[13]  S. Mednick,et al.  CONTINUAL ASSOCIATION AS A FUNCTION OF LEVEL OF CREATIVITY AND TYPE OF VERBAL STIMULUS. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[14]  I ROCK,et al.  The role of repetition in associative learning. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.

[15]  I. Bodis-Wollner,et al.  Visual contrast sensitivity , 1988, Neurology.

[16]  G. Clore,et al.  The affective regulation of cognitive priming. , 2008, Emotion.

[17]  E. Halgren,et al.  Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  H Kolb,et al.  Temporal Resolution -- Webvision: The Organization of the Retina and Visual System , 1995 .

[19]  M. Bar,et al.  Associative Activation and Its Relation to Exploration and Exploitation in the Brain , 2016, Psychological science.

[20]  L. Mudrik,et al.  Understanding associative vs. abstract pictorial relations: An ERP study , 2019, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  John R. Anderson A spreading activation theory of memory. , 1983 .

[22]  D. Navon Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  E. Vul,et al.  Functional adaptive sequential testing. , 2010, Seeing and perceiving.

[24]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[25]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[26]  P. Downing,et al.  Interactions Between Visual Working Memory and Selective Attention , 2000, Psychological science.

[27]  M. Bar A cognitive neuroscience hypothesis of mood and depression , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  E. Granholm,et al.  Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[29]  G. Gigerenzer Gut Feelings: The Intelligence of the Unconscious , 2007 .

[30]  Maryanne M. Gobble,et al.  Design Thinking , 2010, The Palgrave Encyclopedia of the Possible.

[31]  E. Diener,et al.  Most People Are Happy , 1996 .

[32]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Motivation and cognitive abilities: an integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition , 1989 .

[33]  L J Williams,et al.  Cognitive Load and the Functional Field of View , 1982, Human factors.

[34]  Emily Balcetis,et al.  See what you want to see: motivational influences on visual perception. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  George Sperling,et al.  The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .

[36]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Search of associative memory. , 1981 .

[37]  J. McCarley,et al.  Executive working memory load does not compromise perceptual processing during visual search: Evidence from additive factors analysis , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[38]  M. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures , 1990, Brain and Cognition.

[39]  R. Kimchi Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: a critical review. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  Karen Gasper,et al.  Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global Versus Local Processing of Visual Information , 2002, Psychological science.

[41]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[42]  H. van Steenbergen,et al.  Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: A review , 2018, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[43]  Paul T. Sowden,et al.  Perceptual learning of luminance contrast detection: specific for spatial frequency and retinal location but not orientation , 2002, Vision Research.

[44]  N. Lavie,et al.  Perceptual load modulates conscious , 2007 .

[45]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting Attention Based on Long-Term Memory Experience , 2006, Neuron.

[46]  Bart Farell,et al.  Psychoph ysica l methods, or how to measure a threshold , and w hy , 1999 .

[47]  M. Benedek,et al.  Associative abilities underlying creativity. , 2012 .

[48]  Y. Trope,et al.  The association between psychological distance and construal level: evidence from an implicit association test. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  W. N. Dember,et al.  Mood and global-local visual processing , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[50]  Marcus E. Raichle,et al.  Suppression of Regional Cerebral Blood during Emotional versus Higher Cognitive Implications for Interactions between Emotion and Cognition , 1998 .

[51]  D L Nelson,et al.  Prior knowledge and memory: the influence of natural category size as a function of intention and distraction. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Specifying attentional top-down influences on subsequent unconscious semantic processing , 2009, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[53]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. , 1977 .

[54]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[55]  Tad T. Brunyé,et al.  Happiness by association: Breadth of free association influences affective states , 2013, Cognition.

[56]  M. Bar,et al.  The effect of mental progression on mood. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.