Adapting to time: Duration channels do not mediate human time perception.

Accurately encoding the duration and temporal order of events is essential for survival and important to everyday activities, from holding conversations to driving in fast-flowing traffic. Although there is a growing body of evidence that the timing of brief events (< 1 s) is encoded by modality-specific mechanisms, it is not clear how such mechanisms register event duration. One approach gaining traction is a channel-based model; this envisages narrowly-tuned, overlapping timing mechanisms that respond preferentially to different durations. The channel-based model predicts that adapting to a given event duration will result in overestimating and underestimating the duration of longer and shorter events, respectively. We tested the model by having observers judge the duration of a brief (600 ms) visual test stimulus following adaptation to longer (860 ms) and shorter (340 ms) stimulus durations. The channel-based model predicts perceived duration compression of the test stimulus in the former condition and perceived duration expansion in the latter condition. Duration compression occurred in both conditions, suggesting that the channel-based model does not adequately account for perceived duration of visual events.

[1]  D. P. Andrews,et al.  APE: Adaptive probit estimation of psychometric functions , 1981 .

[2]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  The oddball effect: perceived duration and predictive coding. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[3]  R. Ivry The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  Ikuya Murakami,et al.  Perceived duration of visual motion increases with speed. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[5]  Hedderik van Rijn,et al.  Decoupling Interval Timing and Climbing Neural Activity: A Dissociation between CNV and N1P2 Amplitudes , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  C. Douglas Creelman,et al.  Human Discrimination of Auditory Duration , 1962 .

[7]  John E. Schlerf,et al.  Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  A. Kohn Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  J. Nachmias The role of virtual standards in visual discrimination , 2006, Vision Research.

[10]  William Curran,et al.  The many directions of time , 2011, Cognition.

[11]  M Concetta Morrone,et al.  Neural mechanisms for timing visual events are spatially selective in real-world coordinates , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.

[13]  Peter Thompson,et al.  Velocity after-effects: The effects of adaptation to moving stimuli on the perception of subsequently seen moving stimuli , 1981, Vision Research.

[14]  R. Ulrich,et al.  On estimating the difference limen in duration discrimination tasks: A comparison of the 2AFC and the reminder task , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  S. Treue,et al.  The response of neurons in areas V1 and MT of the alert rhesus monkey to moving random dot patterns , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Alan Johnston,et al.  Motion-direction specificity for adaptation-induced duration compression depends on temporal frequency. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[17]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  S. Grondin Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[19]  J. Wearden,et al.  When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur? , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  David M. Eagleman,et al.  Neural Correlates of Subsecond Time Distortion in the Middle Temporal Area of Visual Cortex , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  The spatial tuning of adaptation-based time compression. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[23]  J. Robson,et al.  Application of fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Spatially Localized Distortions of Event Time , 2006, Current Biology.

[25]  Dean V Buonomano,et al.  Book Review: How Do We Tell Time? , 2002, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[26]  David Whitaker,et al.  Duration channels mediate human time perception , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[28]  Welber Marinovic,et al.  Separable temporal metrics for time perception and anticipatory actions , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  P. Tse,et al.  Attention and the subjective expansion of time , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  Masamichi J. Hayashi,et al.  Time Adaptation Shows Duration Selectivity in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2015, PLoS biology.

[31]  D. Buonomano,et al.  The neural basis of temporal processing. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[32]  Inci Ayhan,et al.  Retinotopic adaptation-based visual duration compression. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[33]  S. Holm A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure , 1979 .

[34]  William Curran,et al.  Direction-contingent duration compression is primarily retinotopic , 2014, Vision Research.

[35]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[36]  C. Benton,et al.  Viewpoint dependence in adaptation to facial identity , 2006, Vision Research.