Timing of rhythmic movements in patients with cerebellar degeneration
暂无分享,去创建一个
R. B. Ivry | H. Zelaznik | R. Ivry | R. Spencer | J. Schlerf | J. E. Schlerf | R. M. C. Spencer | H. N. Zelaznik
[1] H. Zelaznik,et al. The Cerebellum and Event Timing , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[2] F. W. Cody,et al. The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson's disease. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[3] M G Paulin,et al. Evolution of the cerebellum as a neuronal machine for Bayesian state estimation , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.
[4] H. Zelaznik,et al. Correlations for timing consistency among tapping and drawing tasks: evidence against a single timing process for motor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] Pressing,et al. Error Correction Processes in Temporal Pattern Production. , 1998, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[6] R. Romo,et al. Timing and neural encoding of somatosensory parametric working memory in macaque prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[7] A. Kristofferson,et al. Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses , 1973 .
[8] J. Desmond,et al. Lobular Patterns of Cerebellar Activation in Verbal Working-Memory and Finger-Tapping Tasks as Revealed by Functional MRI , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Alexa Riehle,et al. Context‐related representation of timing processes in monkey motor cortex , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[10] D. Wolpert,et al. Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[11] M Skalej,et al. Autosomal dominant cerebellar ataxia type I. MRI-based volumetry of posterior fossa structures and basal ganglia in spinocerebellar ataxia types 1, 2 and 3. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[12] R Todd Ogden,et al. Adding drift to the decomposition of simple isochronous tapping: an extension of the Wing-Kristofferson model. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] R. Ivry,et al. Anticipatory adjustments in the unloading task: Is an efference copy necessary for learning? , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] Antonio Cerasa,et al. Sensorimotor transduction of time information is preserved in subjects with cerebellar damage , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.
[15] R. Ivry,et al. When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Yasmin L. Hashambhoy,et al. Neural Correlates of Reach Errors , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] R. Ivry,et al. Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[18] M. Mauk,et al. Cerebellar cortex lesions disrupt learning-dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[19] S. Scott. Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[20] S. Keele,et al. Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] M. Shadlen,et al. Representation of Time by Neurons in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of the Macaque , 2003, Neuron.
[22] Javier F. Medina,et al. Timing Mechanisms in the Cerebellum: Testing Predictions of a Large-Scale Computer Simulation , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] D. Wolpert,et al. The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.
[24] H. Zelaznik,et al. Role of the cerebellum in movements: control of timing or movement transitions? , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[25] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Comparison of patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[26] J. Binder,et al. Distributed Neural Systems Underlying the Timing of Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] G. Madison,et al. Variability in isochronous tapping: higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] M. Hallett,et al. International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale for pharmacological assessment of the cerebellar syndrome , 1997, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.
[29] H. Zelaznik,et al. Disrupted Timing of Discontinuous But Not Continuous Movements by Cerebellar Lesions , 2003, Science.
[30] H. Zelaznik,et al. Temporal Precision in Tapping and Circle Drawing Movements at Preferred Rates is Not Correlated: Further Evidence Against Timing as a General-Purpose Ability , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[31] Richard B Ivry,et al. Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.
[32] M. Kawato,et al. Internal forward models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling. , 2003, Progress in brain research.
[33] H. Zelaznik,et al. Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements. , 2002 .
[34] H. Braak,et al. Spinocerebellar ataxias types 2 and 3: degeneration of the precerebellar nuclei isolates the three phylogenetically defined regions of the cerebellum , 2005, Journal of Neural Transmission.
[35] S. Keele,et al. Timing Functions of The Cerebellum , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] R. B. Ivry,et al. Reduced Timing Variability in Patients with Unilateral Cerebellar Lesions during Bimanual Movements , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] Alan M. Wing,et al. Modeling variability and dependence in timing , 1996 .