Timing of rhythmic movements in patients with cerebellar degeneration

A distinction in temporal performance has been identified between two classes of rhythmic movements: those requiring explicit timing of salient events marking successive cycles, i.e., event timing, and continuous movements in which timing is hypothesized to be emergent. Converging evidence in support of this distinction is reviewed, including neuropsychological studies showing that individuals with cerebellar damage are selectively impaired on tasks requiring event timing (e.g., tapping). Recent behavioral evidence in neurologically healthy individuals suggests that for continuous movements (e.g., circle drawing), the initial cycle is marked by a transformation from event to emergent timing, allowing the participant to match their movement rate to an externally defined cycle duration. We report a new experiment in which individuals with cerebellar ataxia produced rhythmic tapping or circle drawing movements. Participants were either paced by a metronome or unpaced. Ataxics showed a disproportionate increase in temporal variability during tapping compared to circle drawing, although they were more variable than controls on both tasks. However, two predictions of the transformation hypothesis were not confirmed. First, the ataxics did not show a selective impairment on circle drawing during the initial cycles, a phase when we hypothesized event timing would be required to establish the movement rate. Second, the metronome did not increase variability of the performance of the ataxies. Taken together, these results provide further evidence that the integrity of the cerebellum is especially important for event timing, although our attempt to specify the relationship between event and emergent timing was not successful.

[1]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  The Cerebellum and Event Timing , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  F. W. Cody,et al.  The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson's disease. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  M G Paulin,et al.  Evolution of the cerebellum as a neuronal machine for Bayesian state estimation , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.

[4]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Correlations for timing consistency among tapping and drawing tasks: evidence against a single timing process for motor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Pressing,et al.  Error Correction Processes in Temporal Pattern Production. , 1998, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[6]  R. Romo,et al.  Timing and neural encoding of somatosensory parametric working memory in macaque prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses , 1973 .

[8]  J. Desmond,et al.  Lobular Patterns of Cerebellar Activation in Verbal Working-Memory and Finger-Tapping Tasks as Revealed by Functional MRI , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Alexa Riehle,et al.  Context‐related representation of timing processes in monkey motor cortex , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models in the cerebellum , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  M Skalej,et al.  Autosomal dominant cerebellar ataxia type I. MRI-based volumetry of posterior fossa structures and basal ganglia in spinocerebellar ataxia types 1, 2 and 3. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  R Todd Ogden,et al.  Adding drift to the decomposition of simple isochronous tapping: an extension of the Wing-Kristofferson model. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  R. Ivry,et al.  Anticipatory adjustments in the unloading task: Is an efference copy necessary for learning? , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Antonio Cerasa,et al.  Sensorimotor transduction of time information is preserved in subjects with cerebellar damage , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.

[15]  R. Ivry,et al.  When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Yasmin L. Hashambhoy,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reach Errors , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  R. Ivry,et al.  Callosotomy patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during continuous bimanual movements , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  M. Mauk,et al.  Cerebellar cortex lesions disrupt learning-dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  S. Scott Optimal feedback control and the neural basis of volitional motor control , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  S. Keele,et al.  Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Time by Neurons in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of the Macaque , 2003, Neuron.

[22]  Javier F. Medina,et al.  Timing Mechanisms in the Cerebellum: Testing Predictions of a Large-Scale Computer Simulation , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.

[24]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Role of the cerebellum in movements: control of timing or movement transitions? , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Comparison of patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  J. Binder,et al.  Distributed Neural Systems Underlying the Timing of Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  G. Madison,et al.  Variability in isochronous tapping: higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  M. Hallett,et al.  International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale for pharmacological assessment of the cerebellar syndrome , 1997, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[29]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Disrupted Timing of Discontinuous But Not Continuous Movements by Cerebellar Lesions , 2003, Science.

[30]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Temporal Precision in Tapping and Circle Drawing Movements at Preferred Rates is Not Correlated: Further Evidence Against Timing as a General-Purpose Ability , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  Richard B Ivry,et al.  Timing Variability in Circle Drawing and Tapping: Probing the Relationship Between Event and Emergent Timing , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  M. Kawato,et al.  Internal forward models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[33]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements. , 2002 .

[34]  H. Braak,et al.  Spinocerebellar ataxias types 2 and 3: degeneration of the precerebellar nuclei isolates the three phylogenetically defined regions of the cerebellum , 2005, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[35]  S. Keele,et al.  Timing Functions of The Cerebellum , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  R. B. Ivry,et al.  Reduced Timing Variability in Patients with Unilateral Cerebellar Lesions during Bimanual Movements , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Alan M. Wing,et al.  Modeling variability and dependence in timing , 1996 .