Morphological processing of Chinese compounds from a grammatical view

ABSTRACT In the present study, morphological structure processing of Chinese compounds was explored using a visual priming lexical decision task among 21 Hong Kong college students. Two compounding structures were compared. The first type was the subordinate, in which one morpheme modifies the other (e.g., 籃 球 [laam4 kau4, basket-ball, basketball]), similar to most English compounds (e.g., a snowman is a man made of snow and toothpaste is a paste for teeth; the second morpheme is the “head,” modified morpheme). The second type was the coordinative, in which both morphemes contribute equally to the meaning of the word. An example in Chinese is 花 草 (faa1 cou2, flower grass, i.e., plant). There are virtually no examples of this type in English, but an approximate equivalent phrase might be in and out, in which neither in nor out is more important than the other in comprising the expression. For the subordinate Chinese compound words, the same structure in prime and target facilitated the semantic priming effect, whereas for coordinative Chinese compound words, the same structure across prime and target inhibited the semantic priming effect. Results suggest that lexical processing of Chinese compounds is influenced by compounding structure processing.

[1]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Interactive-activation as a framework for understanding morphological processing , 1994 .

[2]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Morphological Structure in the Chinese Mental Lexicon , 1995 .

[3]  Gary Libben,et al.  Compound fracture: The role of semantic transparency and morphological headedness , 2003, Brain and Language.

[4]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. , 1994 .

[5]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Morphology, Orthography, and Phonology in Reading Chinese Compound Words , 1999 .

[6]  Tianwei Xie,et al.  A corpus-based study of character and bigram frequencies in Chinese e-texts and its implications for Chinese language instruction , 2004 .

[7]  Xuchu Weng,et al.  Semantic processing of Chinese in left inferior prefrontal cortex studied with reversible words , 2004, NeuroImage.

[8]  M. Taft Prefix Stripping Revisited. , 1981 .

[9]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  Morphological Awareness and Learning to Read: A Cross-Language Perspective , 2006 .

[10]  Yunheng Ji MORPHOLOGY , 1937, A Grammar of Italian Sign Language (LIS).

[11]  Pienie Zwitserlood,et al.  The role of semantic transparency in the processing and representation of Dutch compounds , 1994 .

[12]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  Compound Nouns and Category Structure in Young Children. , 1985 .

[13]  R. Baayen,et al.  The balance of storage and computation in morphological processing: the role of word formation type, affixal homonymy, and productivity. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  Jun Da A corpus-based study of character and bigram frequencies in Chinese e-texts and its implications for Chinese language instruction 1 , 2004 .

[15]  C. Fowler,et al.  Relations among regular and irregular morphologically related words in the lexicon as revealed by repetition priming , 1985, Memory & cognition.