Simulated Reference Frame: A Cost-Effective Solution to Improve Spatial Orientation in VR
暂无分享,去创建一个
Bernhard E. Riecke | Wolfgang Stuerzlinger | Thinh Nguyen-Vo | B. Riecke | W. Stuerzlinger | Thinh Nguyen-Vo
[1] Eugenia M. Kolasinski,et al. Simulator Sickness in Virtual Environments. , 1995 .
[2] Timothy P. McNamara,et al. Do We Need to Walk for Effective Virtual Reality Navigation? Physical Rotations Alone May Suffice , 2010, Spatial Cognition.
[3] Desney S. Tan,et al. Physically large displays improve performance on spatial tasks , 2006, TCHI.
[4] Bernhard E. Riecke,et al. Moving in a box: Improving spatial orientation in virtual reality using simulated reference frames , 2017, 2017 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI).
[5] J Reason,et al. Motion sickness: Some theoretical and practical considerations. , 1978, Applied ergonomics.
[6] Bernhard E. Riecke,et al. Upper Body Leaning can affect Forward Self-Motion Perception in Virtual Environments , 2015, SUI.
[7] Robert S. Kennedy,et al. Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. , 1993 .
[8] James Carifio,et al. Resolving the 50‐year debate around using and misusing Likert scales , 2008, Medical education.
[9] Behrang Keshavarz,et al. Validating an Efficient Method to Quantify Motion Sickness , 2011, Hum. Factors.
[10] Mark H. Draper,et al. The adaptive effects of virtual interfaces: vestibulo-ocular reflex and simulator sickness , 1998 .
[11] C C Presson,et al. Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.
[12] Timothy P. McNamara,et al. How Are the Locations of Objects in the Environment Represented in Memory? , 2003, Spatial Cognition.
[13] Jonathan W. Kelly,et al. Facilitated pointing to remembered objects in front: Evidence for egocentric retrieval or for spatial priming? , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[14] Anthony E. Richardson,et al. Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.
[15] Ben D. Lawson,et al. Motion Sickness Symptomatology and Origins , 2014, Handbook of Virtual Environments, 2nd ed..
[16] Robert W. Lindeman,et al. On Your Feet!: Enhancing Vection in Leaning-Based Interfaces through Multisensory Stimuli , 2016, SUI.
[17] Timothy P. McNamara,et al. Reference Frames Influence Spatial Memory Development within and Across Sensory Modalities , 2010, Spatial Cognition.
[18] Bing Wu,et al. The embodied actor in multiple frames of reference , 2012 .
[19] J. Rieser. Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[20] G. Norman. Likert scales, levels of measurement and the “laws” of statistics , 2010, Advances in health sciences education : theory and practice.
[21] Bernhard E. Riecke,et al. Can physical motions prevent disorientation in naturalistic VR? , 2012, 2012 IEEE Virtual Reality Workshops (VRW).
[22] R. Klatzky,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Use of Cognitive Versus Perceptual Heading During Imagined Locomotion Depends on the Response Mode , 2022 .
[23] T. McNamara,et al. Where you are affects what you can easily imagine: Environmental geometry elicits sensorimotor interference in remote perspective taking , 2017, Cognition.
[24] Simon Lessels,et al. For Efficient Navigational Search, Humans Require Full Physical Movement, but Not a Rich Visual Scene , 2006, Psychological science.
[25] Thomas A. Furness,et al. The role of rest frames in vection, presence and motion sickness , 1998 .
[26] Kristopher J. Blom,et al. A New Gaming Device and Interaction Method for a First-Person-Shooter , 2005 .
[27] Jason Jerald,et al. The VR Book: Human-Centered Design for Virtual Reality , 2015 .
[28] Kristopher J. Blom,et al. Intuitive, Hands-free Travel Interfaces for Virtual Environments , 2005 .
[29] R. Wang. Between reality and imagination: When is spatial updating automatic? , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al. Perceiving simulated ego-motions in virtual reality: comparing large screen displays with HMDs , 2005, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.
[31] Carman Neustaedter,et al. NaviChair: Evaluating an Embodied Interface Using a Pointing Task to Navigate Virtual Reality , 2015, SUI.
[32] John H. Bailey,et al. Virtual spaces and real world places: transfer of route knowledge , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..
[33] Jack M. Loomis,et al. Locomotion Mode Affects the Updating of Objects Encountered During Travel: The Contribution of Vestibular and Proprioceptive Inputs to Path Integration , 1998, Presence.
[34] Roy A. Ruddle,et al. Movement Around Real and Virtual Cluttered Environments , 2005, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.
[35] Sarah S. Chance,et al. Spatial Updating of Self-Position and Orientation During Real, Imagined, and Virtual Locomotion , 1998 .
[36] Changhoon Park,et al. Effects of rest frames on cybersickness and oscillatory brain activity , 2013, 2013 International Winter Workshop on Brain-Computer Interface (BCI).
[37] Julie M. Drexler,et al. Research in visually induced motion sickness. , 2010, Applied ergonomics.
[38] Kay M. Stanney,et al. Virtual Environment Usage Protocols , 2014, Handbook of Virtual Environments, 2nd ed..