Turning on the alarm: The neural mechanisms of the transition from innocuous to painful sensation

The experience of pain occurs when the level of a stimulus is sufficient to elicit a marked affective response, putatively to warn the organism of potential danger and motivate appropriate behavioral responses. Understanding the biological mechanisms of the transition from innocuous to painful levels of sensation is essential to understanding pain perception as well as clinical conditions characterized by abnormal relationships between stimulation and pain response. Thus, the primary objective of this study was to characterize the neural response associated with this transition and the correspondence between that response and subjective reports of pain. Towards this goal, this study examined BOLD response profiles across a range of temperatures spanning the pain threshold. 14 healthy adults underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while a range of thermal stimuli (44-49°C) were applied. BOLD responses showed a sigmoidal profile along the range of temperatures in a network of brain regions including insula and mid-cingulate, as well as a number of regions associated with motor responses including ventral lateral nuclei of the thalamus, globus pallidus and premotor cortex. A sigmoid function fit to the BOLD responses in these regions explained up to 85% of the variance in individual pain ratings, and yielded an estimate of the temperature of steepest transition from non-painful to painful heat that was nearly identical to that generated by subjective ratings. These results demonstrate a precise characterization of the relationship between objective levels of stimulation, resulting neural activation, and subjective experience of pain and provide direct evidence for a neural mechanism supporting the nonlinear transition from innocuous to painful levels along the sensory continuum.

[1]  A. Apkarian,et al.  Parsing pain perception between nociceptive representation and magnitude estimation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  I. Tracey,et al.  The insula: A multidimensional integration site for pain , 2007, Pain.

[3]  Allan I. Basbaum,et al.  Central nervous system mechanisms of pain modulation , 2006 .

[4]  R. Peyron,et al.  Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000) , 2000, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[5]  Massieh Moayedi,et al.  Is the insula the "how much" intensity coder? , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  A. Schnitzler,et al.  Differential coding of pain intensity in the human primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  R. Bandler,et al.  Columnar organization in the midbrain periaqueductal gray and the integration of emotional expression. , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[8]  R. Davidson,et al.  The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  T. Cook,et al.  Pain ratings at the thresholds are necessary for interpretation of quantitative sensory testing , 2005, Muscle & nerve.

[10]  Tom Johnstone,et al.  Regional response differences across the human amygdaloid complex during social conditioning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  R. Gracely Pain measurement , 1999, Acta anaesthesiologica Scandinavica.

[12]  Jörn Lötsch,et al.  Separating brain processing of pain fromthat of stimulus intensity , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[13]  J. Maisog,et al.  Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  J. Dostrovsky,et al.  Differential projections of thermoreceptive and nociceptive lamina I trigeminothalamic and spinothalamic neurons in the cat. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Robert C. Coghill,et al.  Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  C. Büchel,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses Related to Pain Intensity, Stimulus Intensity, and Stimulus Awareness within the Anterior Cingulate Cortex: A Parametric Single-Trial Laser Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  David Yarnitsky,et al.  Heat pain thresholds: normative data and repeatability , 1995, Pain.

[18]  Angela R Laird,et al.  Brain activity associated with painfully hot stimuli applied to the upper limb: A meta‐analysis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[19]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[20]  A. Craig A rat is not a monkey is not a human: comment on Mogil (Nature Rev. Neurosci. 10, 283–294 (2009)) , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  Tom Johnstone,et al.  Motion correction and the use of motion covariates in multiple‐subject fMRI analysis , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[22]  C Büchel,et al.  Painful stimuli evoke different stimulus-response functions in the amygdala, prefrontal, insula and somatosensory cortex: a single-trial fMRI study. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  A. Craig,et al.  Pain mechanisms: labeled lines versus convergence in central processing. , 2003, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  A. Petrie,et al.  Individuality in pain and suffering , 1967 .

[25]  P. Wall,et al.  Textbook of pain , 1989 .

[26]  Katja Wiech,et al.  Flexible cerebral connectivity patterns subserve contextual modulations of pain. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  V. Neugebauer,et al.  Differential sensitization of amygdala neurons to afferent inputs in a model of arthritic pain. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Anthony K. P. Jones,et al.  Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity , 1997, Pain.

[29]  K. Wiech,et al.  Anterior Insula Integrates Information about Salience into Perceptual Decisions about Pain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Ulf Lindblom,et al.  CLASSIFICATION OF CHRONIC PAIN , 2004 .

[31]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Validating cluster size inference: random field and permutation methods , 2003, NeuroImage.

[32]  E. Rolls The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[33]  Claus C. Hilgetag,et al.  Sequence of information processing for emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex and amygdala , 2007, NeuroImage.