Selective writing impairment: Beyond the allographic code

Abstract A single subject (MP) with a selective writing impairment persisting after recovery from transcortical sensory aphasia due to left parieto-occipital haemorrhage, was evaluated with reference to current models of writing. His writing deficit was localized to the post-graphemic stages and more specifically, to the stage in which the allographic code is transferred to the graphic motor pattern store. A detailed error analysis, with attention to graphic motor complexity and letter frequency, enabled us to specify the factors influencing this transfer process and provides new insight into the neuropsychological mechanisms involved in writing.

[1]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[2]  S. Kohn The nature of the phonemic string deficit in conduction aphasia , 1989 .

[3]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  The rites of righting writing: Homophone remediation in acquired dysgraphia , 1987 .

[4]  K. Heilman,et al.  Dissociations of writing and praxis: Two cases in point , 1986, Brain and Language.

[5]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Dissociation of Spelling Errors in Written and Oral Spelling: The Role of Allographic Conversion in Writing , 1986 .

[6]  K. Heilman,et al.  Linguistic semantic agraphia: A dissociation of the lexical spelling system from semantics , 1986, Brain and Language.

[7]  F. M. Hatfield Visual and phonological factors in acquired dysgraphia , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Cognitive analysis of a case of pure dysgraphia , 1985, Brain and Language.

[9]  D. Margolin The neuropsychology of Writing and Spelling: Semantic, Phonological, Motor, and Perceptual Processes , 1984, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  A. Wing,et al.  Agraphia and micrographia: clinical manifestations of motor programming and performance disorders. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[11]  Kenneth M Heilman,et al.  Apractic agraphia in a patient with normal praxis , 1983, Brain and Language.

[12]  A. Kertesz The Western Aphasia Battery , 1982 .

[13]  Connie Juel,et al.  THE FREQUENCY AND VERSATILITY OF INITIAL AND TERMINAL LETTERS IN ENGLISH WORDS , 1982 .

[14]  A R Damasio,et al.  Praxis and writing of the left hand may be served by different callosal pathways. , 1981, Archives of neurology.

[15]  M. Alexander,et al.  Pure agraphia and unilateral optic ataxia associated with a left superior parietal lobule lesion , 1981, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[16]  K E Patterson,et al.  Neuropsychological approaches to the study of reading. , 1981, British journal of psychology.

[17]  J Dérouesné,et al.  Lexical or orthographic agraphia. , 1981, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  K. Heilman,et al.  Alexia and agraphia with spared spelling and letter recognition abilities , 1981, Brain and Language.

[19]  L. Vignolo,et al.  Dissociated disorders of speaking and writing in aphasia. , 1978, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  Robert L. Solso,et al.  Frequency and versatility of letters in the English language , 1976 .

[21]  Marcel Kinsbourne,et al.  Agraphia selective for written spelling: An experimental case study , 1974 .

[22]  Murray Eden,et al.  Experiments on Computer Recognition of Connected Handwritten Words , 1964, Inf. Control..

[23]  C. Barry,et al.  A cognitive analysis of an acquired dysgraphic patient with an “allographic” writing disorder , 1989 .

[24]  Hatfield Fm,et al.  Interpretation of spelling disorders in aphasia: impact of recent developments in cognitive psychology. , 1984 .

[25]  G. V. Galen,et al.  35 Handwriting and Drawing: A Two Stage Model of Complex Motor Behavior , 1980 .

[26]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Slips of the Pen. , 1979 .