Motion-aftereffect-induced blindness.

Motion-induced blindness (MIB) describes the occasional disappearance of salient visual objects in the presence of moving features (Y. S. Bonneh, A. Cooperman, & D. Sagi, 2001). Here we test whether motion adaptation and the ensuing motion aftereffect (MAE) are sufficient to trigger disappearance of salient targets. In three experiments, observers adapted to either rotating or static stimuli. Immediately afterwards, a static test pattern was presented consisting of a mask with texture elements and three superimposed target dots in a triangular arrangement. Observers reported dot disappearance and reappearance. The results clearly show that illusory motion in a static test pattern, following motion adaptation, promotes the disappearance of target dots. Furthermore, disappearance is modulated by the depth relationship between test pattern and targets, increasing for targets placed stereoscopically behind the test pattern. We conclude that MIB is influenced by perceived relative motion between depth-segregated features.

[1]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  An Invitation to cognitive science , 1997 .

[2]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Visual surface representation: a critical link between lower-level and higher level vision , 1995 .

[3]  H. P. Bowditch,et al.  Optical Illusions of Motion , 1882, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  J. Pettigrew,et al.  A Common Oscillator for Perceptual Rivalries? , 2003, Perception.

[5]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Nulling the motion aftereffect with dynamic random-dot stimuli: limitations and implications. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[6]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Neural Correlates of Motion-induced Blindness in the Human Brain , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  S. Nishida,et al.  Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.

[8]  W. Müller Introduction and historical overview , 2005 .

[9]  Su-Ling Yeh,et al.  A common mechanism for perceptual filling-in and motion-induced blindness , 2006, Vision Research.

[10]  Su-Ling Yeh,et al.  Linking motion-induced blindness to perceptual filling-in , 2004, Vision Research.

[11]  S. Thompson OPTICAL ILLUSIONS OF MOTION , 1880 .

[12]  A. Chaudhuri Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention , 1990, Nature.

[13]  Dov Sagi,et al.  Opposite Neural Signatures of Motion-Induced Blindness in Human Dorsal and Ventral Visual Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Pettigrew,et al.  Does Interhemispheric Competition Mediate Motion-Induced Blindness? A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2003, Perception.

[15]  C. M. D. de Weert,et al.  On Interocular Transfer of Motion Aftereffects , 1993, Perception.

[16]  Erich W Graf,et al.  Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion , 2002, Vision Research.

[17]  R. Andersen,et al.  Integration of motion and stereopsis in middle temporal cortical area of macaques , 1995, Nature.

[18]  R. Blake,et al.  Interocular Transfer of Expansion, Rotation, and Translation Motion Aftereffects , 1994, Perception.

[19]  S M Anstis,et al.  Movement Aftereffects Contingent on Binocular Disparity , 1974, Perception.

[20]  Dov Sagi,et al.  Motion-induced blindness in normal observers , 2001, Nature.

[21]  M Nawrot,et al.  Neural integration of information specifying structure from stereopsis and motion. , 1989, Science.

[22]  H. Barlow,et al.  Evidence for a Physiological Explanation of the Waterfall Phenomenon and Figural After-effects , 1963, Nature.

[23]  K Nakayama,et al.  Binocular visual surface perception. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Perceptual “Read-Out” of Conjoined Direction and Disparity Maps in Extrastriate Area MT , 2004, PLoS biology.

[25]  A. Dale,et al.  Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, Nature.

[26]  D. Heeger,et al.  Neuronal Basis of the Motion Aftereffect Reconsidered , 2001, Neuron.

[27]  K Nakayama,et al.  Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces. , 1992, Science.

[28]  J Aitken A New Variety of Ocular Spectrum. , 1879, Journal of anatomy and physiology.

[29]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Motion Aftereffect:A Modern Perspective , 1998 .

[30]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  A Transparent Motion Aftereffect Contingent on Binocular Disparity , 1994, Perception.

[31]  R Blake,et al.  Another perspective on the visual motion aftereffect. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  John Aitken 2. On a New Variety of Ocular Spectrum. , 1880 .

[33]  J. Bakin,et al.  Visual Responses in Monkey Areas V1 and V2 to Three-Dimensional Surface Configurations , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  T. S. Lee,et al.  Dynamics of subjective contour formation in the early visual cortex. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Attentional modulation of adaptation to two-component transparent motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[36]  Fang Fang,et al.  Strong influence of test patterns on the perception of motion aftereffect and position. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[37]  D. Regan,et al.  Disparity Detectors in Human Depth Perception: Evidence for Directional Selectivity , 1973, Science.

[38]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  Orthogonal motion after-effect illusion predicted by a model of cortical motion processing , 1996, Nature.

[39]  T. R. Scott,et al.  Directional asymmetry of motion aftereffect. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression , 2009, Current Biology.

[41]  James Campbell Quick,et al.  Creating healthier workplaces: The American Psychological Association/National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health cooperative agreement. Introduction and historical overview. , 1997, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[42]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985, Psychological review.

[43]  H. Barlow,et al.  Inter-ocular Transfer of Movement After-effects during Pressure Blinding of the Stimulated Eye , 1963, Nature.

[44]  A. Seiffert,et al.  Motion aftereffects specific to surface depth order: beyond binocular disparity. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[45]  R. Addams LI. An account of a peculiar optical phænomenon seen after having looked at a moving body , 1834 .

[46]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Recovery from motion adaptation is delayed by successively presented orthogonal motion , 1994, Vision Research.

[47]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Independent Aftereffects of Attention and Motion , 2000, Neuron.