Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic-level categories in young infants.

A series of experiments examined the abilities of 3- and 4-month-old infants to form categorical representations to exemplars of natural kinds--cats and horses. These experiments also permitted assessment of the relative exclusivity of these representations--the extent to which they exclude exemplars from contrasting basic-level categories from the same superordinate category. We found that categorical representations could be formed for horses that excluded cats, zebras, and giraffes, and for cats that excluded horses and tigers but not female lions. Lions were, however, excluded from the representations of cats in 6- and 7-month-old infants. Evidence was also obtained for 2 a priori preferences for members of one category over another. The discussion examined the roles of similarity between the exemplars of the contrasting categories and those of the tested category on estimates of exclusivity and of spontaneous preferences on the formation and exclusivity of categorical representations.

[1]  E. Gibson Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1969 .

[2]  J. Fagan Memory in the infant. , 1970, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  Daniel F. Chambliss,et al.  The relative contributions of common and distinctive information on the abstraction from ill-defined categories , 1975 .

[4]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  E. Rosch,et al.  Categorization of Natural Objects , 1981 .

[6]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Categories and concepts , 1984 .

[7]  P. C. Bomba,et al.  The development of orientation categories between 2 and 4 months of age. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[8]  C. Mervis On the existence of prelinguistic categories: A case study , 1985 .

[9]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[10]  Paul C. Quinn,et al.  On Categorization in Early Infancy. , 1986 .

[11]  A lower boundary for category formation in preverbal infants. , 1987, Journal of child language.

[12]  Paul C. Quinn,et al.  The categorical representation of visual pattern information by young infants , 1987, Cognition.

[13]  J. Reznick,et al.  The Development of Contextual Categories , 1987 .

[14]  K. Roberts Retrieval of a basic-level category in prelinguistic infants. , 1988 .

[15]  J. Mandler,et al.  The cradle of categorization: Is the basic level basic? , 1988 .

[16]  R. A. Haaf,et al.  Observer accuracy and observer agreement in the measurement of visual fixation with fixed-trial procedures , 1989 .

[17]  Perception and its neuronal mechanisms , 1989, Cognition.

[18]  K. Roberts,et al.  Categorization studies of 9- to 15-month-old infants: Evidence for superordinate categorization? , 1989 .

[19]  G. Edelman,et al.  Signal and sense : local and global order in perceptual maps , 1990 .

[20]  R. Giora Concepts and conceptual development: Ulric Neisser, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1987. 317 pp , 1990 .

[21]  R. Nosofsky Tests of an exemplar model for relating perceptual classification and recognition memory. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Kelly L. Madole,et al.  Infants' object examining: Habituation and categorization ☆ ☆☆ , 1991 .

[23]  J. Mandler,et al.  Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[24]  J. Tanaka,et al.  Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  J. Mandler How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. , 1992, Psychological review.

[26]  F. Keil Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development , 1989 .

[27]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Evidence for Representations of Perceptually Similar Natural Categories by 3-Month-Old and 4-Month-Old Infants , 1993, Perception.