Neural processes of attentional inhibition of return traced with magnetoencephalography

Inhibition of return (IOR) is a phenomenon that involves reaction times (RTs) to a spatially cued target that are longer than RTs to an uncued target when the interval between the cue and target is prolonged. Although numerous studies have examined IOR, no consensus has yet been reached regarding the neural mechanisms responsible for it. We used magnetoencephalography (MEG) and measured the human neural responses underlying the time course of IOR, applying a typical spatial cueing paradigm. The cue-target interval was 600+/-200 ms. Three experimental conditions were employed. Cued; the cue and target were presented at the same location. Uncued; the two stimuli were presented at opposite locations. Neutral; the cue stimulus was presented bilaterally. We found differences in the amplitudes of signals in the postero-temporal and bilateral temporal areas, and peak latencies in a central area between the cued and uncued conditions. These signals were localized to the extrastriate cortex, bilateral temporal-parietal junction (TPJ), and primary motor cortex, respectively. Bilateral TPJ activities are related to the identification of salient events in the sensory environment both within and independent of the current behavioral context and may play an important role in IOR in addition to extrastriate and the primary motor cortex.

[1]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Occipital-parietal interactions during shifts of exogenous visuospatial attention: trial-dependent changes of effective connectivity. , 2004, Magnetic Resonance Imaging.

[2]  R. Hari,et al.  Cortical Visuomotor Integration during Eye Pursuit and Eye–Finger Pursuit , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Signal-space projection method for separating MEG or EEG into components , 1997, Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing.

[4]  L. Anllo-Vento Shifting attention in visual space: the effects of peripheral cueing on brain cortical potentials. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[5]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Sensory gating as a physiological mechanism for visual selective attention. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[6]  Masanobu Ito Fractionated Reaction Time as a Function of Magnitude of Force in Simple and Choice Conditions , 1997, Perceptual and motor skills.

[7]  R. Kakigi,et al.  Magnetic response of human extrastriate cortex in the detection of coherent and incoherent motion , 2000, Neuroscience.

[8]  Rob H. J. van der Lubbe,et al.  Different Effects of Exogenous Cues in a Visual Detection and Discrimination Task: Delayed Attention Withdrawal and/or Speeded Motor Inhibition? , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[10]  H Suzuki,et al.  Human cortical area responding to stimuli in apparent motion , 1997, Neuroreport.

[11]  Antigona Martínez,et al.  Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Magnetoencephalography-theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain , 1993 .

[13]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Audiovisual Integration of Letters in the Human Brain , 2000, Neuron.

[14]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Inhibition of Return From Stimulus to Response , 2004, Psychological science.

[15]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Matthias Schlesewsky,et al.  Detection of early cognitive processing by event-related phase synchronization analysis , 2005, Neuroreport.

[17]  Carrie J. McAdams,et al.  Effects of Attention on the Reliability of Individual Neurons in Monkey Visual Cortex , 1999, Neuron.

[18]  Andrew R. Mayer,et al.  Neural networks underlying endogenous and exogenous visual–spatial orienting , 2004, NeuroImage.

[19]  M. Eimer An ERP study on visual spatial priming with peripheral onsets. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[20]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Cortical expressions of inhibition of return , 2006, Brain Research.

[21]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  Visual detection of motion speed in humans: spatiotemporal analysis by fMRI and MEG , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[23]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Visual Search has Memory , 2001, Psychological science.

[24]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  R. Knight,et al.  Neural origins of the P300. , 2000, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[26]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[27]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  Edmund Wascher,et al.  Revealing effects of noninformative spatial cues: an EEG study of inhibition of return. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[29]  H. Freund,et al.  Magneto-encephalographic correlates of the lateralized readiness potential. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[30]  Matti S. Hämäläinen,et al.  Functional localization based on measurements with a whole-head magnetometer system , 2005, Brain Topography.

[31]  Andrew R. Mayer,et al.  An Event-related fMRI Study of Exogenous Orienting: Supporting Evidence for the Cortical Basis of Inhibition of Return? , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  S. Pollmann,et al.  Covert Reorienting and Inhibition of Return: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[34]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[36]  R. Knight,et al.  Effects of temporal-parietal lesions on the somatosensory P3 to lower limb stimulation. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  J. Botwinick,et al.  Premotor and motor components of reaction time. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  Albert Gjedde,et al.  Origin of Human Motor Readiness Field Linked to Left Middle Frontal Gyrus by MEG and PET , 1998, NeuroImage.

[41]  Lawrence M. Ward,et al.  An event-related brain potential study of inhibition of return , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  J. Pratt,et al.  The role of the fixation location in inhibition of return. , 2000, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[43]  R. Klein,et al.  Contribution of the Primate Superior Colliculus to Inhibition of Return , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Delayed Striate Cortical Activation during Spatial Attention , 2002, Neuron.

[45]  R. Hari,et al.  Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Inhibition of return in spatial attention: direct evidence for collicular generation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  Srikantan S. Nagarajan,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of the Time-order Error: An MEG Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  Kimron Shapiro,et al.  Target consolidation under high temporal processing demands as revealed by MEG , 2005, NeuroImage.

[49]  T. Paus,et al.  Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[50]  R. Hari,et al.  Viewing Lip Forms Cortical Dynamics , 2002, Neuron.

[51]  D. O'Boyle,et al.  The contribution of non-ocular response inhibition to visual inhibition of return , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[53]  Emi Tanaka,et al.  Human cortical response to various apparent motions: A magnetoencephalographic study , 2007, Neuroscience Research.

[54]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[55]  R. Eason,et al.  The conjoint influence of spatial selective attention and motor set on very short latency vers , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Attention Modulates Contextual Influences in the Primary Visual Cortex of Alert Monkeys , 1999, Neuron.

[57]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  M. Eimer “Sensory gating” as a mechanism for visuospatial orienting: Electrophysiological evidence from trial-by-trial cuing experiments , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[59]  C. Kennard,et al.  Distinct Cortical and Collicular Mechanisms of Inhibition of Return Revealed with S Cone Stimuli , 2004, Current Biology.

[60]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  Distinct Neural Correlates for Resolving Stroop Conflict at Inhibited and Noninhibited Locations in Inhibition of Return , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  E. Somersalo,et al.  Visualization of Magnetoencephalographic Data Using Minimum Current Estimates , 1999, NeuroImage.

[62]  J. Downar,et al.  The Effect of Task Relevance on the Cortical Response to Changes in Visual and Auditory Stimuli: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[63]  G R Mangun,et al.  Tracking the influence of reflexive attention on sensory and cognitive processing , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[64]  R. Hari,et al.  Coinciding early activation of the human primary visual cortex and anteromedial cuneus , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[65]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Right TPJ deactivation during visual search: functional significance and support for a filter hypothesis. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[66]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  G Gratton,et al.  Attention and probability effects in the human occipital cortex: an optical imaging study , 1997, Neuroreport.

[68]  J. Gore,et al.  Neural Correlates of the Attentional Blink , 2000, Neuron.