Overconfident: Do You Put Your Money on it?

A group exhibits overconfidence if significantly more than half the group members declare to be better than the median in some characteristic. Overconfidence was found in verbal reports for a variety of characteristics and settings but was less often studied for choice behaviour. In an experiment we tested how perceived relative skill influences verbal and choice behaviour. Treatments varied task difficulty and payment. Choice behaviour changes from overconfidence to underconfidence when the task changes from easy and familiar to non-familiar. This effect is significant when monetary payments are at stake and weak when they are not.

[1]  J. Tirole,et al.  Self-Confidence and Personal Motivation , 2002 .

[2]  Terrance Odean,et al.  Learning to Be Overconfident , 1997 .

[3]  Stephen E. Wilcox Investor Psychology and Security Market Under- and Overreactions , 1999 .

[4]  M. L. Klotz,et al.  Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .

[5]  D. Dunning,et al.  Ambiguity and self-evaluation: the role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability , 1989 .

[6]  J. Eiser,et al.  Optimism, Pessimism, and the Direction of Self–Other Comparisons , 2001 .

[7]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Road user behavior and traffic accidents , 1976 .

[8]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Overconfidence and Excess Entry: An Experimental Approach , 1999 .

[9]  Colin Camerer,et al.  The Effects of Financial Incentives in Experiments: A Review and Capital-Labor-Production Framework , 1999 .

[10]  Ola Svenson,et al.  Risks of road transportation in a psychological perspective , 1978 .

[11]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[12]  M. Alicke Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. , 1985 .

[13]  A. Rustichini,et al.  A Fine is a Price , 2000, The Journal of Legal Studies.

[14]  A. Tversky,et al.  The weighing of evidence and the determinants of confidence , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[15]  Y. Klar,et al.  No one in my group can be below the group's average: a robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  I. W. Molenaar,et al.  Rasch models: foundations, recent developments and applications , 1995 .

[17]  V. Smith,et al.  Monetary rewards and decision cost in experimental economics , 2000 .

[18]  S Harris,et al.  Psychology of drivers in traffic accidents. , 1965, The Journal of applied psychology.

[19]  J. Tirole,et al.  Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation , 2003 .

[20]  R. Thaler,et al.  Financial Decision-Making in Markets and Firms: A Behavioral Perspective , 1994 .

[21]  L. Perloff,et al.  Self–other judgments and perceived vulnerability to victimization. , 1986 .

[22]  J. Kruger Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Bram P. Buunk,et al.  SOCIAL-COMPARISON OF HEALTH RISKS - LOCUS OF CONTROL, THE PERSON-POSITIVITY BIAS, AND UNREALISTIC OPTIMISM , 1993 .

[24]  A. Tversky,et al.  Preference and belief: Ambiguity and competence in choice under uncertainty , 1991 .

[25]  O. Svenson ARE WE ALL LESS RISKY AND MORE SKILLFUL THAN OUR FELLOW DRIVERS , 1981 .

[26]  David M. Messick,et al.  Why We Are Fairer Than Others , 1985 .

[27]  A. Rustichini,et al.  Pay Enough or Don't Pay at All , 2000 .

[28]  Y. Klar,et al.  Are Most People Happier than their Peers, or Are They Just Happy? , 1999 .

[29]  Baruch Fischhoff,et al.  Accident probabilities and seat belt usage: A psychological perspective☆ , 1978 .