The spatial and temporal dynamics of anticipatory preparation and response inhibition in task-switching

We investigated ERP and fMRI correlates of anticipatory preparation and response inhibition in a cued task-switching paradigm with informatively cued, non-informatively cued and no-go trials. Cue-locked ERPs showed evidence for a multicomponent preparation process. An early cue-locked differential positivity was larger for informative vs. non-informative cues and its amplitude correlated with differential activity for informatively vs. non-informatively cued trials in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), consistent with a goal activation process. A later differential positivity was larger for informatively cued switch vs. repeat trials and its amplitude correlated with informatively cued switch vs. repeat activity in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), compatible with a category-response (C-R) rule activation process. No-go trials elicited a frontal P3, whose amplitude was negatively correlated with activity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and basal ganglia motor network, suggesting that a network responsible for response execution was inhibited in the course of a no-go trial. These findings indicate that anticipatory preparation in task-switching is comprised of at least two processes: goal activation and C-R rule activation. They also support a functional dissociation between DLPFC and VLPFC, with the former involved in top-down biasing and the latter involved in response inhibition.

[1]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Functionally Specific Reorganization in Human Premotor Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[2]  D. E. Astle,et al.  Dissociating neural indices of dynamic cognitive control in advance task-set preparation: An ERP study of task switching , 2006, Brain Research.

[3]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Marcel Brass,et al.  When the same response has different meanings: recoding the response meaning in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[6]  M. Brass,et al.  Internally generated and directly cued task sets: an investigation with fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  Alexander Hammers,et al.  Volumes, spatial extents and a probabilistic atlas of the human basal ganglia and thalamus , 2007, NeuroImage.

[9]  Dorothee P. Auer,et al.  Neural networks of response shifting: Influence of task speed and stimulus material , 2006, Brain Research.

[10]  C. Liston,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and Posterior Parietal Cortices Are Sensitive to Dissociable Forms of Conflict in a Task-Switching Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[11]  R. Barry,et al.  Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[12]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Mapping the functional anatomy of task preparation: Priming task‐appropriate brain networks , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[13]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  Modulation of brain activity by selective task sets observed using event-related potentials , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Rugg,et al.  Interpreting event-related brain potentials. , 2004 .

[15]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  Response priming in the Go/NoGo task: The N2 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[16]  R. Douglas,et al.  The hippocampus and behavior. , 1967, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  Thomas R. Knösche,et al.  Who Comes First? The Role of the Prefrontal and Parietal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Neural Evidence for Representation-Specific Response Selection , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Brass,et al.  Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  STN Stimulation Alters Pallidal—Frontal Coupling during Response Selection under Competition , 2007, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[21]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Effects of response selection on the task repetition benefit in task switching , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[22]  Gregory V. Simpson,et al.  Preparatory allocation of attention and adjustments in conflict processing , 2007, NeuroImage.

[23]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Modelling Geometric Deformations in Epi Time Series , 2022 .

[24]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Predicting Success: Patterns of Cortical Activation and Deactivation Prior to Response Inhibition , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  J. Zacks Neuroimaging Studies of Mental Rotation: A Meta-analysis and Review , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  S. Swinnen,et al.  The missing link between action and cognition , 2007, Progress in Neurobiology.

[27]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Anticipatory reconfiguration elicited by fully and partially informative cues that validly predict a switch in task , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  G. Winocur,et al.  Hippocampus and septum in response inhibition. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[29]  T. Goschke Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .

[30]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory and poststimulus components of task switching. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[31]  Birte U. Forstmann,et al.  Neural Mechanisms, Temporal Dynamics, and Individual Differences in Interference Control , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[33]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  M Spitzer,et al.  The time course of brain activations during response inhibition: evidence from event‐related potentials in a go/no go task , 1998, Neuroreport.

[36]  Juliana Yordanova,et al.  On the relation of movement-related potentials to the go/no-go effect on P3 , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[37]  M. Brass,et al.  Advance preparation and stimulus-induced interference in cued task switching: further insights from BOLD fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Paul D. Kieffaber,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of task switching and switch costs. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[39]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[41]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[42]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[43]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Parietal cortex and attention , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[44]  Daniel M. Corcos,et al.  Three-dimensional locations and boundaries of motor and premotor cortices as defined by functional brain imaging: A meta-analysis , 2006, NeuroImage.

[45]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[46]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes , 2000 .

[47]  John C Gore,et al.  Integration of fMRI, NIROT and ERP for studies of human brain function. , 2006, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[48]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[49]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  Components of task-set reconfiguration: Differential effects of ‘switch-to’ and ‘switch-away’ cues , 2006, Brain Research.

[51]  Francisco Barceló,et al.  Spatiotemporal brain dynamics during preparatory set shifting: MEG evidence , 2004, NeuroImage.

[52]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  Rüdiger J. Seitz,et al.  Functional clusters in the human parietal cortex as revealed by an observer-independent meta-analysis of functional activation studies , 2005, Anatomy and Embryology.

[54]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Equivalence of cognitive processes in brain imaging and behavioral studies: evidence from task switching , 2003, NeuroImage.

[55]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[56]  Martin P. Paulus,et al.  Superior temporal gyrus and insula provide response and outcome-dependent information during assessment and action selection in a decision-making situation , 2005, NeuroImage.

[57]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Inhibition-Related ERP Components: Variation with Modality, Age, and Time-on-Task , 2002 .

[58]  U Hegerl,et al.  Event-related potentials: Do they reflect central serotonergic neurotransmission and do they predict clinical response to serotonin agonists? , 2001 .

[59]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[60]  H. Steinbusch,et al.  The functional role of the subthalamic nucleus in cognitive and limbic circuits , 2005, Progress in Neurobiology.

[61]  J. Tanji,et al.  Distinctions between dorsal and ventral premotor areas: anatomical connectivity and functional properties , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[62]  G. Jackson,et al.  Behavioural and neurophysiological correlates of bivalent and univalent responses during task switching , 2007, Brain Research.

[63]  E. Halgren,et al.  Spatiotemporal mapping of brain activity by integration of multiple imaging modalities , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[64]  D. Guthrie,et al.  Significance testing of difference potentials. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[65]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Components of Switching Intentional Set , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[67]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Caffeine improves anticipatory processes in task switching , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[68]  J. Thayer,et al.  The continuing problem of false positives in repeated measures ANOVA in psychophysiology: a multivariate solution. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[69]  Frini Karayanidis,et al.  ERPs dissociate the effects of switching task sets and task cues , 2006, Brain Research.

[70]  C. Price The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimaging , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[71]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Short cue presentations encourage advance task preparation: a recipe to diminish the residual switch cost. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[72]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[73]  G. Glover,et al.  Acquiring and inhibiting prepotent responses in schizophrenia: event-related brain potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[74]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[75]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[76]  N. McNaughton The role of the subiculum within the behavioural inhibition system , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[77]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[78]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[79]  Rachel Swainson,et al.  Fractionating the Cognitive Control Required to Bring About a Change in Task: A Dense-sensor Event-related Potential Study , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Neural mechanisms for response selection: comparing selection of responses and items from working memory , 2007, NeuroImage.

[81]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Jumping the gun: is effective preparation contingent upon anticipatory activation in task-relevant neural circuitry? , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[82]  A. Parent,et al.  Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. I. The cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop , 1995, Brain Research Reviews.

[83]  Tobias Bast,et al.  Toward an Integrative Perspective on Hippocampal Function: From the Rapid Encoding of Experience to Adaptive Behavior , 2007, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[84]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Neurophysiological signature of effective anticipatory task‐set control: a task‐switching investigation , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[85]  Trichur R. Vidyasagar,et al.  Functional comparison of neuronal properties in the primate posterior hippocampus and parahippocampus (area TF/TH) during different behavioural paradigms involving memory and selective attention , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[86]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[87]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[88]  Gregory V. Simpson,et al.  Evidence for Anterior Cingulate Cortex Involvement in Monitoring Preparatory Attentional Set , 2002, NeuroImage.

[89]  A. Nambu,et al.  Functional significance of the cortico–subthalamo–pallidal ‘hyperdirect’ pathway , 2002, Neuroscience Research.

[90]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory task-switching processes. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[91]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[92]  Adam P. Morris,et al.  Executive Brake Failure following Deactivation of Human Frontal Lobe , 2006 .

[93]  Phan Luu,et al.  Dynamics of task sets: evidence from dense-array event-related potentials. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[94]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching , 2000 .

[95]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[96]  A. Parent,et al.  Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. II. The place of subthalamic nucleus and external pallidium in basal ganglia circuitry , 1995, Brain Research Reviews.

[97]  I. Koch,et al.  The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[98]  A C Nobre,et al.  Components of attentional set-switching. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[99]  M. Kawato,et al.  Different neural correlates of reward expectation and reward expectation error in the putamen and caudate nucleus during stimulus-action-reward association learning. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[100]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  Sequence effects in cued task switching modulate response preparedness and repetition priming processes. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[101]  Mathias Bartels,et al.  Pausing for thought: engagement of left temporal cortex during pauses in speech , 2004, NeuroImage.

[102]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[103]  S. R. Jackson,et al.  Using advance information in dynamic cognitive control: An ERP study of task-switching , 2006, Brain Research.

[104]  C. Li,et al.  Activation of the pre-supplementary motor area but not inferior prefrontal cortex in association with short stop signal reaction time – an intra-subject analysis , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[105]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  Switching between univalent task-sets in schizophrenia: ERP evidence of an anticipatory task-set reconfiguration deficit , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[106]  Natalie A. Phillips,et al.  Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[107]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[108]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[109]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  Compensatory mechanisms underlie intact task-switching performance in schizophrenia , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[110]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[111]  A. Allport,et al.  Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[112]  M. Merello,et al.  [Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia]. , 2000, Revista de neurologia.

[113]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[114]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Event-related fMRI , 1997 .

[115]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Cognitive control involved in overcoming prepotent response tendencies and switching between tasks. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.