Combining vision with audition and touch, in adults and in children
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Roberto Arrighi,et al. Meaningful auditory information enhances perception of visual biological motion. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[2] G. Sandini,et al. Cross-modal facilitation of visual and tactile motion , 2008 .
[3] R. Wurtz. Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability , 2008, Vision Research.
[4] David C. Burr,et al. Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information , 2008, Current Biology.
[5] Pete R. Jones,et al. Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation , 2008, Current Biology.
[6] Wei Ji Ma,et al. Spiking networks for Bayesian inference and choice , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[7] A. Billard,et al. Auditory motion affects visual biological motion processing , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[8] David C Burr,et al. Fusion of Visual and Auditory Stimuli during Saccades: A Bayesian Explanation for Perisaccadic Distortions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Jess Hartcher-O’Brien,et al. Temporal Ventriloquism: Perceptual shifts forwards and backwards in time predicted by the maximum likelihood model , 2007 .
[10] Robert H. Wurtz,et al. Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex , 2006, Nature.
[11] C. Granrud,et al. Development of size constancy in children: A test of the proximal mode sensitivity hypothesis , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[12] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Development of multisensory spatial integration and perception in humans. , 2006, Developmental science.
[13] David Alais,et al. No direction-specific bimodal facilitation for audiovisual motion detection. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[14] Terry Caelli,et al. Development of configural 3D object recognition , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.
[15] D. Burr,et al. The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.
[16] G F Meyer,et al. The integration of auditory and visual motion signals at threshold , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[17] Paul Bertelson,et al. Temporal ventriloquism: crossmodal interaction on the time dimension. 2. Evidence from sensorimotor synchronization. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[18] G. Aschersleben,et al. Temporal ventriloquism: crossmodal interaction on the time dimension. 1. Evidence from auditory-visual temporal order judgment. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[19] Tracey D. Berger,et al. Flicker flutter: is an illusory event as good as the real thing? , 2003, Journal of vision.
[20] Robert A Jacobs,et al. Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[21] A. Kingstone,et al. Auditory capture of vision: examining temporal ventriloquism. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[22] Frank Bremmer,et al. Neural Correlates of Visual Localization and Perisaccadic Mislocalization , 2003, Neuron.
[23] J. Gunn. Dumbstruck: A Cultural History of Ventriloquism , 2003 .
[24] A. Streri,et al. Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes in human newborns , 2003, Somatosensory & motor research.
[25] G. Essick,et al. Tactile motion activates the human middle temporal/V5 (MT/V5) complex , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.
[26] C. Spence,et al. Tactile “capture” of audition , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] Kae Nakamura,et al. Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[28] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[29] S. Shimojo,et al. Sound alters visual evoked potentials in humans , 2001, Neuroreport.
[30] M. Wallace,et al. Sensory and Multisensory Responses in the Newborn Monkey Superior Colliculus , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] S. Wuerger,et al. Cross-modal integration of auditory and visual motion signals , 2001, Neuroreport.
[32] A. Pouget,et al. Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] L. Bahrick,et al. Increasing specificity in perceptual development: infants' detection of nested levels of multimodal stimulation. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[34] D. Burr,et al. Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.
[35] S. Shimojo,et al. Illusions: What you see is what you hear , 2000, Nature.
[36] M. Morrone,et al. Extraretinal Control of Saccadic Suppression , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] D. Lewkowicz,et al. The development of intersensory temporal perception: an epigenetic systems/limitations view. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.
[38] I. Kovács,et al. Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[39] R. Jacobs,et al. Optimal integration of texture and motion cues to depth , 1999, Vision Research.
[40] D. Burr,et al. Vision: Modular analysis – or not? , 1999, Current Biology.
[41] A. Dale,et al. Functional analysis of primary visual cortex (V1) in humans. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[42] M. Concetta Morrone,et al. Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[43] Pietro G. Morasso,et al. Self-Organization, Computational Maps, and Motor Control , 1997 .
[44] W. Simpson,et al. Pedestal effect in visual motion discrimination. , 1995, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[45] D. Burr,et al. Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.
[46] D. Lewkowicz. Infants’ responsiveness to the auditory and visual attributes of a sounding/moving stimulus , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[47] J R Duhamel,et al. The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.
[48] James J. Clark,et al. Data Fusion for Sensory Information Processing Systems , 1990 .
[49] D R Perrott,et al. Minimum audible angle thresholds for sources varying in both elevation and azimuth. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[50] S Appelle,et al. Eliminating the Haptic Oblique Effect: Influence of Scanning Incongruity and Prior Knowledge of the Standards , 1986, Perception.
[51] S. Mateeff,et al. Dynamic Visual Capture: Apparent Auditory Motion Induced by a Moving Visual Target , 1985, Perception.
[52] D G Pelli,et al. Uncertainty explains many aspects of visual contrast detection and discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[53] R. Watt,et al. The recognition and representation of edge blur: Evidence for spatial primitives in human vision , 1983, Vision Research.
[54] A. Watson,et al. Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[55] D. Burr,et al. Selective depression of motion sensitivity during saccades. , 1982, The Journal of physiology.
[56] D. H. Warren,et al. The role of visual-auditory “compellingness” in the ventriloquism effect: Implications for transitivity among the spatial senses , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[57] J Nachmias,et al. Letter: Grating contrast: discrimination may be better than detection. , 1974, Vision research.
[58] B E Stein,et al. Sequence of changes in properties of neurons of superior colliculus of the kitten during maturation. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.
[59] S. Appelle. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.
[60] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[61] L. Matin,et al. Visual Perception of Direction for Stimuli Flashed During Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.
[62] T SHIPLEY,et al. Auditory Flutter-Driving of Visual Flicker , 1964, Science.
[63] A. Mills. On the minimum audible angle , 1958 .
[64] H. Leibowitz,et al. Apparent visual size as a function of distance for children and adults. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.
[65] S. Basu,et al. Determinants of Perception , 2009 .
[66] M. Morrone,et al. Spatio-temporal distortions of visual perception during saccades , 2007 .
[67] J. Haxby,et al. Perception of visual and tactile flow activates common cortical areas in the human brain , 2004 .
[68] Franco Lepore,et al. Comparison of sensitivity to first- and second-order local motion in 5-year-olds and adults. , 2003, Spatial vision.
[69] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Computational models of sensorimotor integration , 1997 .
[70] M T Wallace,et al. The visually responsive neuron and beyond: multisensory integration in cat and monkey. , 1993, Progress in brain research.
[71] J. Atkinson,et al. Human visual development over the first 6 months of life. A review and a hypothesis. , 1984, Human neurobiology.
[72] G. Gottlieb. Development of species identification in birds : an inquiry into the prenatal determinants of perception , 1971 .
[73] G H MOWBRAY,et al. On discriminating the rate of visual flicker and auditory flutter. , 1959, The American journal of psychology.