High-density mapping in an N400 paradigm: evidence for bilateral temporal lobe generators

OBJECTIVE The aim of this study was to obtain a more detailed description of N400 scalp topography than has previously been reported. METHODS High-density (128 channel) visual event-related potentials were measured in an N400 paradigm using semantically incongruous sentence endings. RESULTS The stimuli elicited an N400 with a centroparietal scalp distribution. In addition, P400s with similar timing and functional characteristics were observed at non-standard recording locations inferior to the temporal lobes. CONCLUSIONS The data are consistent with intracranial evidence for bilateral activation of anterior medial temporal lobe structures. These structures are oriented such that the positive regions of their scalp fields lie largely outside of the area sampled by standard electrode montages. P400s at other non-standard scalp locations, including infraorbital and infraoccipital sites, may reflect volume conduction from the same generators, or activation of non-temporal lobe generators.

[1]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: II. Effects of word type and semantic priming , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words , 1980, Biological Psychology.

[3]  M. Balish,et al.  The inverse problem in electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. , 1990, Advances in neurology.

[4]  John Crosbie,et al.  A simple Turbo Pascal 4.0 program for millisecond timing on the IBM PC/XT/AT , 1989 .

[5]  A. Puce,et al.  Visual recognition memory. Neurophysiological evidence for the role of temporal white matter in man. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related potential asymmetries during the reading of sentences. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[7]  M. Kutas,et al.  Views on how the electrical activity that the brain generates reflects the functions of different language structures. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[8]  M I Posner,et al.  Topography of the N400: brain electrical activity reflecting semantic expectancy. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  Klaus Lehnertz,et al.  Human temporal lobe potentials in verbal learning and memory processes , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  D. Tucker Spatial sampling of head electrical fields: the geodesic sensor net. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[11]  B. N'Kaoua,et al.  Intracranial topography of event-related potentials (N400/P600) elicited during a continuous recognition memory task. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[12]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind. , 1996 .

[13]  E. Halgren,et al.  Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 2. Depth-recorded potentials in the human frontal and Rolandic cortices , 1994, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[14]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Language-related field potentials in the anterior-medial temporal lobe: I. Intracranial distribution and neural generators , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[16]  O Bertrand,et al.  A theoretical justification of the average reference in topographic evoked potential studies. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  E. Halgren,et al.  Neuronal activity in the human medial temporal lobe during recognition memory. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  E. Halgren,et al.  Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing. 1. Depth recorded potentials in the human occipital and parietal lobes , 1994, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[19]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[21]  P. Holcomb,et al.  Event related potentials and language comprehension. , 1995 .

[22]  C Tomberg,et al.  Emulation of somatosensory evoked potential (SEP) components with the 3-shell head model and the problem of 'ghost potential fields' when using an average reference in brain mapping. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[23]  M. Kutas,et al.  The Search for Common Sense: An Electrophysiological Study of the Comprehension of Words and Pictures in Reading , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Salmelin,et al.  Distinct time courses of word and context comprehension in the left temporal cortex. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Arnold B. Scheibel,et al.  Neurobiology of higher cognitive function , 1990 .

[26]  E. Halgren,et al.  Human medial temporal lobe potentials evoked in memory and language tasks. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Language-Related ERPs: Scalp Distributions and Modulation by Word Type and Semantic Priming , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Ray Johnson,et al.  Event-related brain potentials : basic issues and applications , 1990 .

[29]  Joseph Dien,et al.  Issues in the application of the average reference: Review, critiques, and recommendations , 1998 .

[30]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Screen control and timing routines for the IBM microcomputer family using a high-level language , 1988 .