Human Neuroscience Hypothesis and Theory Article Two Systems of Non-symbolic Numerical Cognition
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Fei Xu,et al. Linking Visual Attention and Number Processing in the Brain: The Role of the Temporo-parietal Junction in Small and Large Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Number Comparison , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] R. Church,et al. A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[3] N. Lavie. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[4] P. Cavanagh,et al. The Capacity of Visual Short-Term Memory is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects , 2004, Psychological science.
[5] Christian N. L. Olivers,et al. Subitizing requires attention , 2008 .
[6] Andrea Facoetti,et al. Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.
[7] N. Cowan. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[8] A. Anastasi. Individual differences. , 2020, Annual review of psychology.
[9] Chang Liu,et al. Can subitizing survive the attentional blink? An ERP study , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.
[10] Yaoda Xu,et al. Distinctive Neural Mechanisms Supporting Visual Object Individuation and Identification , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Thomas K. Landauer,et al. Determinants of reaction time for digit inequality judgments , 1973 .
[12] Marco Dadda,et al. Quantity discrimination in female mosquitofish , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[13] S. Dehaene,et al. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .
[14] Daniel C. Hyde,et al. All Numbers Are Not Equal: An Electrophysiological Investigation of Small and Large Number Representations , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[15] Daniel C. Hyde,et al. Small and large number discrimination in guppies , 2011, Animal Cognition.
[16] Melanie Palomares,et al. The Effect of Viewing Eccentricity on Enumeration , 2011, PloS one.
[17] W. Ma,et al. A detection theory account of change detection. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[18] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[19] Bahador Bahrami,et al. A Candidate for the Attentional Bottleneck: Set-size Specific Modulation of the Right TPJ during Attentive Enumeration , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] Susan Carey,et al. What representations might underlie infant numerical knowledge , 1999 .
[21] D. Burr,et al. A Visual Sense of Number , 2007, Current Biology.
[22] David C Burr,et al. Adaptation affects both high and low (subitized) numbers under conditions of high attentional load. , 2011, Seeing and perceiving.
[23] M. Chun,et al. Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects , 2006, Nature.
[24] R. Rafal,et al. Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[25] R. Church,et al. Alternative representations of time, number, and rate , 1990, Cognition.
[26] E. Vogel,et al. Interactions between attention and working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.
[27] Karen Wynn,et al. Infants’ auditory enumeration: Evidence for analog magnitudes in the small number range , 2009, Cognition.
[28] S. Luck,et al. Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory , 2008, Nature.
[29] D. Melcher,et al. Subitizing reflects visuo-spatial object individuation capacity , 2011, Cognition.
[30] Andreas Nieder,et al. A Labeled-Line Code for Small and Large Numerosities in the Monkey Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] Justin Halberda,et al. Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.
[32] Elizabeth S Spelke,et al. Spatiotemporal dynamics of processing nonsymbolic number: An event‐related potential source localization study , 2012, Human brain mapping.
[33] Sara Cordes,et al. Crossing the divide: infants discriminate small from large numerosities. , 2009, Developmental psychology.
[34] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Discrimination of small numerosities in young chicks. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[35] S. Carey,et al. On the limits of infants' quantification of small object arrays , 2005, Cognition.
[36] J. B.,et al. The Power of Numerical Discrimination , 1871, Nature.
[37] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[38] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[39] H S Terrace,et al. Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.
[40] S. Carey,et al. The Representations Underlying Infants' Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes , 2002, Psychological science.
[41] L. Feigenson,et al. Multiple Spatially Overlapping Sets Can Be Enumerated in Parallel , 2006, Psychological science.
[42] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.
[43] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Discrimination of Large and Small Numerosities by Human Infants , 2004 .
[44] Marco Zorzi,et al. A Computational Model of Number Comparison , 2020, Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
[45] Wim Fias,et al. Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] Daniel Ansari,et al. Small and large number processing in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. , 2008, Developmental science.
[47] Elizabeth S Spelke,et al. Neural signatures of number processing in human infants: evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition. , 2011, Developmental science.
[48] Manuela Piazza,et al. Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[49] Nilli Lavie,et al. Contrasting effects of sensory limits and capacity limits in visual selective attention , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] Susan J. Hespos,et al. Do infants understand simple arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992) ☆ , 1995 .
[51] Justin N Wood,et al. Infants' enumeration of actions: numerical discrimination and its signature limits. , 2005, Developmental science.
[52] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[53] David C Burr,et al. Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[54] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Imprinted numbers: newborn chicks' sensitivity to number vs. continuous extent of objects they have been reared with. , 2010, Developmental science.
[55] Edward K. Vogel,et al. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.
[56] S. Dehaene,et al. Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.
[57] Daniel Ansari,et al. Individual differences in children’s mathematical competence are related to the intentional but not automatic processing of Arabic numerals , 2011, Cognition.
[58] Brian Butterworth,et al. The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .
[59] Carly J. Leonard,et al. The role of attention in subitizing: Is the magical number 1? , 2008 .
[60] Daniel C. Hyde,et al. Spatial Attention Determines the Nature of Nonverbal Number Representation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[61] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Does Subitizing Reflect Numerical Estimation? , 2022 .
[62] Susan Carey,et al. One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.
[63] E. L. Kaufman,et al. The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.
[64] 永福 智志. The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[65] Fei Xu,et al. Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations , 2003, Cognition.
[66] Daniel Ansari,et al. Linking visual attention and number processing in the brain : the role of the right temporo-parietal junction in small and large non-symbolic number comparison , 2007 .
[67] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling , 2010, Cognition.
[68] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attention Response Functions Characterizing Brain Areas Using fMRI Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load , 2001, Neuron.
[69] Lisa Feigenson,et al. Tracking individuals via object-files: evidence from infants' manual search , 2003 .
[70] Bahador Bahrami,et al. Modulating Attentional Load Affects Numerosity Estimation: Evidence against a Pre-Attentive Subitizing Mechanism , 2008, PloS one.
[71] Brian J. Scholl. Objects and attention , 2002 .
[72] J. Cantlon,et al. Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans , 2006, Psychological science.
[73] S. Carey. The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .
[74] P. Cavanagh,et al. Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[75] Rochel Gelman,et al. Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[76] E. Vogel,et al. Neural Measures of Individual Differences in Selecting and Tracking Multiple Moving Objects , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[77] E. Spelke,et al. Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .