Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits

Although humans can direct their attention to visual targets with or without eye movements, it remains unclear how different brain mechanisms control visual attention and eye movements together and/or separately. Here, we measured MEG and fMRI data during covert/overt visual pursuit tasks and estimated cortical currents using our previously developed extra-dipole, hierarchical Bayesian method. Then, we predicted the time series of target positions and velocities from the estimated cortical currents of each task using a sparse machine-learning algorithm. The predicted target positions/velocities had high temporal correlations with actual visual target kinetics. Additionally, we investigated the generalization ability of predictive models among three conditions: control, covert, and overt pursuit tasks. When training and testing data were the same tasks, the largest reconstructed accuracies were overt, followed by covert and control, in that order. When training and testing data were selected from different tasks, accuracies were in reverse order. These results are well explained by the assumption that predictive models consist of combinations of three computational brain functions: visual information-processing, maintenance of attention, and eye-movement control. Our results indicate that separate subsets of neurons in the same cortical regions control visual attention and eye movements differently.

[1]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  Allocation of attention during pursuit of large objects is no different than during fixation. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[2]  M. Shaw Time to move on. , 2008, International journal of epidemiology.

[3]  M. Posner The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Orienting of Attention: Then and Now , 2022 .

[4]  Daniel T. Smith,et al.  Associations and Dissociations between Oculomotor Readiness and Covert Attention , 2019, Vision.

[5]  Clayton E Curtis,et al.  Cortical activity time locked to the shift and maintenance of spatial attention. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Yasuharu Koike,et al.  Reconstruction of flexor and extensor muscle activities from electroencephalography cortical currents , 2012, NeuroImage.

[7]  C. Rorden,et al.  Covert orienting of attention and overt eye movements activate identical brain regions , 2008, Brain Research.

[8]  Janis P Bellack,et al.  Then and now. , 2011, The Journal of nursing education.

[9]  Y. Okada,et al.  Genesis of MEG signals in a mammalian CNS structure. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  Masa-aki Sato,et al.  Hierarchical Bayesian estimation for MEG inverse problem , 2004, NeuroImage.

[11]  Michael E. Tipping,et al.  Analysis of Sparse Bayesian Learning , 2001, NIPS.

[12]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Selection and Maintenance of Spatial Information by Frontal Eye Field Neurons , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Masa-aki Sato,et al.  Online Model Selection Based on the Variational Bayes , 2001, Neural Computation.

[14]  Kenji Kawano,et al.  Initiation of smooth pursuit in humans , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Neuronal Basis of Covert Spatial Attention in the Frontal Eye Field , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Experimental and computational analysis of monkey smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2007, Neuron.

[18]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  A model of smooth pursuit in primates based on learning the target dynamics , 2005, Neural Networks.

[19]  Masa-aki Sato,et al.  Reconstruction of two-dimensional movement trajectories from selected magnetoencephalography cortical currents by combined sparse Bayesian methods , 2011, NeuroImage.

[20]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  R. Krauzlis Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  A. Seth,et al.  Granger Causality Analysis in Neuroscience and Neuroimaging , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Masa-aki Sato,et al.  Estimation of hyper-parameters for a hierarchical model of combined cortical and extra-brain current sources in the MEG inverse problem , 2014, NeuroImage.

[25]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[26]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it is overtly ignored , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Mark W. Greenlee,et al.  Connectivity modulation of early visual processing areas during covert and overt tracking tasks , 2008, NeuroImage.

[28]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  S. Baillet,et al.  Localization of realistic cortical activity in MEG using current multipoles , 2004, NeuroImage.

[30]  Masaki Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Multiple Top–Down Signals during Covert Tracking of Moving Objects in Macaque Prefrontal Cortex , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  R. C. Macridis A review , 1963 .

[32]  Richard J. Krauzlis,et al.  Spatial allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2009, Vision Research.

[33]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.

[34]  Jan Friso Groote,et al.  20 Years After , 2007 .

[35]  T. Schenk,et al.  The Premotor theory of attention: Time to move on? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  J. Gottlieb,et al.  Distinct neural mechanisms of distractor suppression in the frontal and parietal lobe , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  V. Glauche,et al.  Gaze pursuit, ‘attention pursuit’ and their effects on cortical activations , 2007 .

[38]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  A Parametric fMRI Study of Overt and Covert Shifts of Visuospatial Attention , 2001, NeuroImage.

[39]  Beau Dabbs,et al.  Summary and discussion of : “ Controlling the False Discovery Rate : A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing , 2014 .

[40]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  When Are Attention and Saccade Preparation Dissociated? , 2009, Psychological science.

[41]  Karl J. Friston Functional and Effective Connectivity: A Review , 2011, Brain Connect..

[42]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Magnetoencephalography-theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain , 1993 .

[43]  E. Miller,et al.  Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices , 2007, Science.

[44]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.

[45]  Yasuhito Sawahata,et al.  Spatial smoothing hurts localization but not information: Pitfalls for brain mappers , 2010, NeuroImage.

[46]  Masa-aki Sato,et al.  Evaluation of hierarchical Bayesian method through retinotopic brain activities reconstruction from fMRI and MEG signals , 2008, NeuroImage.

[47]  C. Cierpka,et al.  Particle imaging techniques for volumetric three-component (3D3C) velocity measurements in microfluidics , 2011, Journal of Visualization.

[48]  Peter Thier,et al.  The neural basis of smooth pursuit eye movements in the rhesus monkey brain , 2008, Brain and Cognition.