Foraging intention affects whether willow tits call to attract members of mixed-species flocks
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] William Hoppitt,et al. Interspecific social networks promote information transmission in wild songbirds , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[2] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[3] P. Petraitis,et al. Inferring multiple causality : the limitations of path analysis , 1996 .
[4] M. Oommen,et al. Shrewd alliances: mixed foraging associations between treeshrews, greater racket-tailed drongos and sparrowhawks on Great Nicobar Island, India , 2010, Biology Letters.
[5] I. Krams. Communication in crested tits and the risk of predation , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[6] A. Brodin. Theoretical models of adaptive energy management in small wintering birds , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[7] Todd M. Freeberg,et al. Chick-a-dee call variation in Carolina chickadees and recruiting flockmates to food , 2009 .
[8] J. Altmann,et al. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.
[9] Donald E. Kroodsma,et al. Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[10] Food Storing Behaviour of Willow Tit Parus montanus , 1988 .
[11] L. Sasvári. Observational learning in great, blue and marsh tits , 1979, Animal Behaviour.
[12] Erick Greene,et al. Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[13] Kathryn E. Sieving,et al. Antipredator calls of tufted titmice and interspecific transfer of encoded threat information , 2012 .
[14] Michael L. Avery,et al. The versatility of graded acoustic measures in classification of predation threats by the tufted titmouse Baeolophus bicolor: exploring a mixed framework for threat communication , 2010 .
[15] Todd M. Freeberg,et al. Variation in Note Composition of Chick-a-dee Calls is Associated with Signaler Flight in Carolina Chickadees, Poecile carolinensis , 2013 .
[16] D. Bates,et al. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.
[17] D. Farine,et al. Social networks predict patch discovery in a wild population of songbirds , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[18] Tom V. Smulders,et al. Hyperdispersed cache distributions reduce pilferage: a field study , 2007, Animal Behaviour.
[19] M. Elgar. House sparrows establish foraging flocks by giving chirrup calls if the resources are divisible , 1986, Animal Behaviour.
[20] Stuart Bearhop,et al. The importance of fission–fusion social group dynamics in birds , 2014 .
[21] T. Hino. Mutualistic and commensal organization of avian mixed-species foraging flocks in a forest of western Madagascar , 1998 .
[22] J. Rappole,et al. Kleptoparasitism of laughingthrushes Garrulax by Greater Racket-tailed Drongos Dicrurus paradiseus in Myanmar , 2001 .
[23] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Long-Distance Calling by the Willow Tit, Poecile montanus, Facilitates Formation of Mixed-Species Foraging Flocks , 2012 .
[24] E. Goodale,et al. Vocal mimicry by a passerine bird attracts other species involved in mixed-species flocks , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[25] H. Schielzeth. Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients , 2010 .
[26] Kartik Shanker,et al. Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? A large-scale synthesis , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[27] L. Carrascal. FOOD CACHING VERSUS IMMEDIATE CONSUMPTION IN THE NUTHATCH: THE EFFECT OF SOCIAL CONTEXT , 1993 .
[28] R. R. Krausz. Living in Groups , 2013 .
[29] Graeme D Ruxton,et al. Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. , 2010, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[30] Z. Emberts,et al. Social Information on Fear and Food Drives Animal Grouping and Fitness , 2017, The American Naturalist.
[31] Damien R. Farine,et al. The role of social and ecological processes in structuring animal populations: a case study from automated tracking of wild birds , 2015, Royal Society Open Science.
[32] T. Freeberg,et al. Diversity in mixed species groups improves success in a novel feeder test in a wild songbird community , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[33] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[34] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Calling at a Food Source: Context-Dependent Variation in Note Composition of Combinatorial Calls in Willow Tits , 2012 .
[35] Jeffrey R. Lucas,et al. “Information” and the chick-a-dee call: Communicating with a complex vocal system , 2007 .
[36] Damien R. Farine,et al. Collective decision making and social interaction rules in mixed-species flocks of songbirds , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[37] J. Lucas,et al. Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[38] D. H. Morse,et al. Ecological Aspects of Some Mixed-Species Foraging Flocks of Birds , 1970 .
[39] John M. Fryxell,et al. Individual Behavior and Community Dynamics , 1997, Population and Community Biology Series.
[40] David Wheatcroft,et al. Species replacement reduces community participation in avian antipredator groups , 2016 .
[41] Erick Greene,et al. Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.
[42] J. Hayashi. [Sampling methods]. , 1982, Josanpu zasshi = The Japanese journal for midwife.
[43] V. Pravosudov. Mountain chickadees discriminate between potential cache pilferers and non-pilferers , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[44] The early bird gets the worm: foraging strategies of wild songbirds lead to the early discovery of food sources , 2013, Biology Letters.
[45] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Referential calls coordinate multi-species mobbing in a forest bird community , 2015, Journal of Ethology.