Motivational control of goal-directed action

The control of goal-directed, instrumental actions by primary motivational states, such as hunger and thirst, is mediated by two processes. The first is engaged by the Pavlovian association between contextual or discriminative stimuli and the outcome or reinforcer presented during instrumental training. Such stimuli exert a motivational influence on instrumental performance that depends upon the relevance of the associated outcome to the current motivational state of the agent. Moreover, the motivational effects of these stimuli operate in the absence of prior experience with the outcome under the relevant motivational state. The second, instrumental, process is mediated by knowledge of the contingency between the action and its outcome and controls the value assigned to this outcome. In contrast to the Pavlovian process, motivational states do not influence the instrumental process directly; rather, the agent has to learn about the value of an outcome in a given motivational state by exposure to it while in that state. This incentive learning is similar in certain respects to the acquisition of “cathexes” envisaged by Tolman (1949a, 1949b).

[1]  B. Skinner On the Rate of Formation of a Conditioned Reflex , 1932 .

[2]  G. C. Grindley THE FORMATION OF A SIMPLE HABIT IN GUINEA-PIGS , 1932 .

[3]  I. P. Pavlov,et al.  The reply of a physiologist to psychologists. , 1932 .

[4]  B. Skinner Conditioning and Extinction and Their Relation to Drive , 1936 .

[5]  R. S. Sackett The effect of strength of drive at the time of extinction upon resistance to extinction in rats. , 1939 .

[6]  G. Heathers,et al.  The relation between strength of drive and rate of extinction of a bar-pressing reaction in the rat. , 1941 .

[7]  C. T. Perin Behavior potentiality as a joint function of the amount of training and the degree of hunger at the time of extinction. , 1942 .

[8]  W. Estes Discriminative conditioning. I. A discriminative property of conditioned anticipation. , 1943 .

[9]  L. S. Kogan Review of Principles of Behavior. , 1943 .

[10]  B. Skinner,et al.  Principles of Behavior , 1944 .

[11]  S. Koch,et al.  The effect of satiation on the behavior mediated by a habit of maximum strength. , 1945 .

[12]  L. Saltzman,et al.  The effect of low intensities of hunger on the behavior mediated by a habit of maximum strength. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  E. Tolman There is more than one kind of learning. , 1949, Psychological review.

[14]  E. Tolman The nature and functioning of wants. , 1949, Psychological review.

[15]  B. Horenstein Performance of conditioned responses as a function of strength of hunger drive. , 1951, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[16]  W. S. Verplanck,et al.  Eating and drinking as a function of maintenance schedule. , 1953, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[17]  W. B. Webb Drive Stimuli as Cues , 1955 .

[18]  G. Grice,et al.  Effect of irrelevant thirst motivation on a response learned with food reward. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  R. Wetzel The effect of experience with a taste reward. , 1959 .

[20]  J. H. Curtis,et al.  Learning Theory and Behavior , 1960 .

[21]  H. M. Jenkins,et al.  Effect of discrimination training on auditory generalization. , 1960, Journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  J. Fitzsimons Drinking by nephrectomized rats injected with various substances , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  Habit Strength as a Function of Drive in a Bar-Pressing Situation , 1961 .

[24]  C. P. Crocetti Drive Level and Response Strength in the Bar-Pressing Apparatus , 1962 .

[25]  Koch Sigmund Ed,et al.  Psychology: A Study of A Science , 1962 .

[26]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[27]  S. Revusky Hunger level during food consumption: Effects on subsequent preference , 1967 .

[28]  E. Fischer Conditioned Reflexes , 1942, American journal of physical medicine.

[29]  R. Bolles Theory of Motivation , 1967 .

[30]  J. Konorski Integrative activity of the brain : an interdisciplinary approach , 1967 .

[31]  S. Morrison,et al.  THE WATER EXCHANGE AND POLYURIA OF RATS DEPRIVED OF FOOD , 1967 .

[32]  S. Ochs Integrative Activity of the Brain: An Interdisciplinary Approach , 1968 .

[33]  E. E. Krieckhaus,et al.  Acquisition of sodium by rats: interaction of innate mechanisms and latent learning. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[34]  S. Revusky Effects of thirst level during consumption of flavored water on subsequent preference. , 1968 .

[35]  J. Konorski,et al.  On a particular form of conditioned reflex. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[36]  J. S. Myer,et al.  Control of food-motivated instrumental behavior in water-deprived rats by prior water and saline drinking , 1970 .

[37]  F. W. Irwin Intentional behavior and motivation : a cognitive theory , 1971 .

[38]  M. Cabanac Physiological Role of Pleasure , 1971, Science.

[39]  R. Bolles Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. , 1972 .

[40]  D. McFarland,et al.  Hydration releases inhibition of feeding produced by intracranial angiotensin. , 1973, Physiology & behavior.

[41]  M. Morgan Resistance to satiation , 1974 .

[42]  É. Asratyan Conditional reflex theory and motivational behavior. , 1974, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[43]  Asratyan Ea Conditional reflex theory and motivational behavior. , 1974 .

[44]  I. Gormezano,et al.  The pavlovian analysis of instrumental conditioning , 1976, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[45]  S. Woods,et al.  Iron appetite and latent learning in rats , 1977, Physiology & Behavior.

[46]  K. Spence Behavior Theory and Conditioning , 1978 .

[47]  D. Bindra How adaptive behavior is produced: a perceptual-motivational alternative to response reinforcements , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[48]  J. R. Hovancik The effect of deprivation level during noncontingent pairings and instrumental learning on subsequent instrumental performance , 1978 .

[49]  Conditioning: An Image Approach , 1979 .

[50]  R. Hendersen,et al.  Avoidance of heat by rats: Effects of thermal context on rapidity of extinction , 1979 .

[51]  J. H. McHose,et al.  The residual deprivation effect in instrumental conditioning as a function of deprivation level at the time of exposure to the reinforcer , 1980 .

[52]  R. Bolles,et al.  Comparisons of stimulus learning and response learning in a punishment situation , 1980 .

[53]  Christopher D. Adams,et al.  Instrumental Responding following Reinforcer Devaluation , 1981 .

[54]  K. Berridge,et al.  Relation of consummatory responses and preabsorptive insulin release to palatability and learned taste aversions. , 1981, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[55]  S. Lea,et al.  Contemporary Animal Learning Theory, Anthony Dickinson. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1981), xii, +177 pp. £12.50 hardback, £3.95 paperback , 1981 .

[56]  T. Davidson,et al.  Resistance to satiation: Reinforcing effects of food and eating under satiation , 1981 .

[57]  T. Davidson,et al.  Deprivation stimulus intensity and incentive factors in the control of instrumental responding. , 1981 .

[58]  E. Capaldi,et al.  Taste preferences as a function of food deprivation during original taste exposure , 1982 .

[59]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Irrelevant Incentive Learning during Instrumental Conditioning: The Role of the Drive-Reinforcer and Response-Reinforcer Relationships , 1983 .

[60]  Elizabeth D. Capaldi,et al.  Conditioned flavor preferences based on hunger level during original flavor exposure , 1983 .

[61]  H. Grill,et al.  Quality of acquired responses to tastes by Rattus norvegicus depends on type of associated discomfort. , 1983, Journal of comparative psychology.

[62]  K. Berridge,et al.  Sodium depletion enhances salt palatability in rats. , 1984, Behavioral neuroscience.

[63]  K. Berridge,et al.  Taste reactivity as a measure of the neural control of palatability , 1985 .

[64]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding. , 1985 .

[65]  A. Dickinson Re-Examination of the Role of the Instrumental Contingency in the Sodium-Appetite Irrelevant Incentive Effect , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[66]  W. Hershberger An approach through the looking-glass , 1986 .

[67]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Associative Structures In Instrumental Learning , 1986 .

[68]  B. Everitt,et al.  Studies of instrumental behavior with sexual reinforcement in male rats (Rattus norvegicus): II. Effects of preoptic area lesions, castration, and testosterone. , 1987, Journal of comparative psychology.

[69]  T. Davidson Learning about deprivation intensity stimuli. , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.

[70]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Pavlovian Processes in the Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance , 1987 .

[71]  A. Dickinson,et al.  The Role of the Instrumental Contingency in the Motivational Control of Performance , 1987 .

[72]  A. Fallon,et al.  A perspective on disgust. , 1987, Psychological review.

[73]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance: The Role of Prior Experience of The Reinforcer , 1988 .

[74]  J. Bradford,et al.  An examination of the relationship between expectancy learning and preference conditioning , 1988 .

[75]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Incentive Learning and the Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance , 1989 .

[76]  A. Dickinson Expectancy theory in animal conditioning. , 1989 .

[77]  J. Garcia Food for Tolman: Cognition and cathexis in concert. , 1989 .

[78]  S. Klein,et al.  Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory , 1991 .

[79]  B. Balleine,et al.  Incentive learning and the motivational control of instrumental performance by thirst , 1989 .

[80]  C. L. Meachum The Role of Response-Contingent Incentives in Lithium Chloride-Mediated Suppression of an Operant Response , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[81]  Frank Baeyens,et al.  Flavor-flavor and color-flavor conditioning in humans , 1990 .

[82]  B. Balleine,et al.  Motivational Control of Instrumental Performance following a Shift from Thirst to Hunger , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[83]  E. Capaldi,et al.  Food deprivation and conditioned flavor preferences based on sweetened and unsweetened foods , 1991 .

[84]  B. Balleine,et al.  Instrumental Performance following Reinforcer Devaluation Depends upon Incentive Learning , 1991 .

[85]  Kent C. Berridge,et al.  Modulation of taste affect by hunger, caloric satiety, and sensory-specific satiety in the rat , 1991, Appetite.

[86]  R. Rescorla Depression of an Instrumental Response by a Single Devaluation of its Outcome , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[87]  B. Balleine,et al.  Incentive Learning Following Reinforcer Devaluation is not Conditional upon the Motivational State during Re-Exposure , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[88]  B. Balleine Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends on incentive learning. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[89]  L. Jarrard,et al.  Potency of food deprivation intensity cues as discriminative stimuli. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[90]  B. Balleine Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends on incentive learning. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[91]  B. Balleine,et al.  Signalling and Incentive Processes in Instrumental Reinforcer Devaluation , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.